Chapter 11. Emotions and Motivations

11. Emotions and Motivations

Grace under Pressure

On June 27, 2014, 13-year-old Gavin England saved his grandfather from drowning when their prawning boat took

on water and sank off the Saanich Peninsula on Vancouver Island (CTV, 2014). Gavin’s grandfather Vern was not

a strong swimmer, and though both were wearing life jackets, they would not have survived for long in the cold

Pacific ocean waters 300 meters from shore.

Gavin recounted the event, explaining how he suffered sharp cuts to his bare feet when climbing the embankment

where he had dragged his grandfather. He attributed his ability to overcome the pain of the cuts to adrenalin. Upon

finding an old truck with keys in the ignition, and despite the high emotions he was experiencing, he then had the

wherewithal to learn to drive on the spot and make it up a three-kilometer hill to get help. Gavin explained that his

knowledge of driving a dirt bike served him well: “I knew that clutch in meant drive.” Vern described the young

boy as “tenacious” and calm throughout the event. He was giving his grandfather words of encouragement as he

pulled him to shore.

Stories such as Gavin’s are rare and unpredictable. We hope we will act with the same clear-headed tenacity in

emergency situations, but the heroic response is not assured. Gavin’s ability to abate panic, and recognize and

regulate his emotions was central to his actions in this emergency situation.

The topic of this chapter is affect, defined as the experience of feeling or emotion. Affect is an essential part of

the study of psychology because it plays such an important role in everyday life. As we will see, affect guides

behaviour, helps us make decisions, and has a major impact on our mental and physical health.

The two fundamental components of affect are emotions and motivation. Both of these words have the same

underlying Latin root, meaning “to move.” In contrast to cognitive processes that are calm, collected, and frequently

rational, emotions and motivations involve arousal, or our experiences of the bodily responses created by the

sympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system (ANS). Because they involve arousal, emotions and

motivations are “hot” — they “charge,” “drive,” or “move” our behaviour.

When we experience emotions or strong motivations, we feel the experiences. When we become aroused, the

sympathetic nervous system provides us with energy to respond to our environment. The liver puts extra sugar into

the bloodstream, the heart pumps more blood, our pupils dilate to help us see better, respiration increases, and we

begin to perspire to cool the body. The stress hormones epinephrine and norepinephrine are released. We experience

these responses as arousal.

American pilot Captain”Sully” Sullenberger (Figure 11.1, “Captain Sullenberger and His Plane on the Hudson

River”) was 915 metres up in the air when the sudden loss of power in his airplane put his life, as well as the lives of

150 passengers and crew members, in his hands. Both of the engines on flight 1539 had shut down, and his options

for a safe landing were limited.

Sully kept flying the plane and alerted the control tower to the situation: “This is Cactus 1539…hit birds. We lost

thrust in both engines. We’re turning back toward La Guardia.”

When the tower gave him the compass setting and runway for a possible landing, Sullenberger’s extensive

430

experience allowed him to give a calm response: “I’m not sure if we can make any runway…Anything in New

Jersey?”

Captain Sullenberger was not just any pilot in a crisis, but a former U.S. Air Force fighter pilot with 40 years of

flight experience. He had served both as a flight instructor and the safety chairman for the Airline Pilots Association.

Training had quickened his mental processes in assessing the threat, allowing him to maintain what tower operators

later called an “eerie calm.” He knew the capabilities of his plane.

When the tower suggested a runway in New Jersey, Sullenberger calmly replied: “We’re unable. We may end up in

the Hudson.”

Figure 11.1 Captain Sullenberger and His Plane on the Hudson River. Imagine that you are on a

plane that you know is going to crash. What emotions would you experience, and how would you

respond to them? Would the rush of fear cause you to panic, or could you control your emotions

like Captain Sullenberger did, as he calmly calculated the heading, position, thrust, and elevation

of the plane, and then landed it on the Hudson River?

The last communication from Captain Sullenberger to the tower advised of the eventual outcome: “We’re going to

be in the Hudson.”

He calmly set the plane down on the water. Passengers reported that the landing was like landing on a rough runway.

The crew kept the passengers calm as women, children, and then the rest of the passengers were evacuated onto

the rescue boats that had quickly arrived. Captain Sullenberger then calmly walked the aisle of the plane to be sure

that everyone was out before joining the 150 other rescued survivors (Levin, 2009; National Transportation Safety

Board, 2009).

Some called it “grace under pressure,” and others called it the “miracle on the Hudson.” But psychologists see it as

the ultimate in emotion regulation — the ability to control and productively use one’s emotions.

An emotion is a mental and physiological feeling state that directs our attention and guides our behaviour. Whether

it is the thrill of a roller-coaster ride that elicits an unexpected scream, the flush of embarrassment that follows a

public mistake, or the horror of a potential plane crash that creates an exceptionally brilliant response in a pilot,

emotions move our actions. Emotions normally serve an adaptive role: We care for infants because of the love we

feel for them, we avoid making a left turn onto a crowded highway because we fear that a speeding truck may hit us,

and we are particularly nice to Mandy because we are feeling guilty that we did not go to her party. But emotions

may also be destructive, such as when a frustrating experience leads us to lash out at others who do not deserve it.

The Surrey School District in British Columbia has incorporated “emotional regulation” into the curriculum (Wells,

431 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

2013). In six schools, educators are piloting a program that helps teachers look for what may be stressing children,

making them unable to pay attention, lethargic, hyperactive, or out of control. The children may be impacted by too

much noise in the classroom, too little sleep, or too much junk food in their lunch. The teachers help the children

recognize what they need to do to make themselves calm and productive in class. The program ultimately places the

motivation for behavioural control within the hands of the children.

Motivations are closely related to emotions. A motivation is a driving force that initiates and directs behaviour.

Some motivations are biological, such as the motivation for food, water, and sex. But there are a variety of other

personal and social motivations that can influence behaviour, including the motivations for social approval and

acceptance, the motivation to achieve, and the motivation to take, or to avoid taking, risks (Morsella, Bargh, &

Gollwitzer, 2009). In each case we follow our motivations because they are rewarding. As predicted by basic

theories of operant learning, motivations lead us to engage in particular behaviours because doing so makes us feel

good.

Motivations are often considered in psychology in terms of drives, which are internal states that are activated when

the physiological characteristics of the body are out of balance, and goals, which are desired end states that we

strive to attain. Motivation can thus be conceptualized as a series of behavioural responses that lead us to attempt

to reduce drives and to attain goals by comparing our current state with a desired end state (Lawrence, Carver, &

Scheier, 2002). Like a thermostat on an air conditioner, the body tries to maintain homeostasis, the natural state of

the body’s systems, with goals, drives, and arousal in balance. When a drive or goal is aroused—for instance, when

we are hungry—the thermostat turns on and we start to behave in a way that attempts to reduce the drive or meet the

goal (in this case to seek food). As the body works toward the desired end state, the thermostat continues to check

whether or not the end state has been reached. Eventually, the need or goal is satisfied (we eat), and the relevant

behaviours are turned off. The body’s thermostat continues to check for homeostasis and is always ready to react to

future needs.

In addition to more basic motivations such as hunger, a variety of other personal and social motivations can also

be conceptualized in terms of drives or goals. When the goal of studying for an exam is hindered because we take

a day off from our schoolwork, we may work harder on our studying on the next day to move us toward our goal.

When we are dieting, we may be more likely to have a big binge on a day when the scale says that we have met

our prior day’s goals. And when we are lonely, the motivation to be around other people is aroused and we try to

socialize. In many, if not most cases, our emotions and motivations operate out of our conscious awareness to guide

our behaviour (Freud, 1922; Hassin, Bargh, & Zimerman, 2009; Williams, Bargh, Nocera, & Gray, 2009).

We begin this chapter by considering the role of affect on behaviour, discussing the most important psychological

theories of emotions. Then we will consider how emotions influence our mental and physical health. We will discuss

how the experience of long-term stress causes illness, and then turn to research on positive thinking and what has

been learned about the beneficial health effects of more positive emotions. Finally, we will review some of the most

important human motivations, including the behaviours of eating and sex. The importance of this chapter is not only

in helping you gain an understanding the principles of affect but also in helping you discover the important roles

that affect plays in our everyday lives, and particularly in our mental and physical health. The study of the interface

between affect and physical health — that principle that “everything that is physiological is also psychological” —

is a key focus of the branch of psychology known as health psychology. The importance of this topic has made

health psychology one of the fastest growing fields in psychology.

References

CTV. (2014). Heroic act (video broadcast). Toronto, ON: CTV National News. Retrieved July 24, 2014, from

http://toronto.ctvnews.ca/video?clipId=389519

11. EMOTIONS AND MOTIVATIONS • 432

Freud, S. (1922). The unconscious. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 56(3), 291.

Hassin, R. R., Bargh, J. A., & Zimerman, S. (2009). Automatic and flexible: The case of nonconscious goal

pursuit. Social Cognition, 27(1), 20–36.

Lawrence, J. W., Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Velocity toward goal attainment in immediate experience

as a determinant of affect. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32(4), 788–802.

Levin, A. (2009, June 9). Experience averts tragedy in Hudson landing. USA Today. Retrieved

from http://www.usatoday.com/news/nation/2009-06-08-hudson_N.htm.

Morsella, E., Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Oxford handbook of human action. New York, NY: Oxford

University Press.

National Transportation Safety Board. (2009, June 9). Excerpts of Flight 1549 cockpit communications. USA

Today. Retrieved from http://www.usatoday.com/news/nation/2009-06-09-hudson-cockpit-transcript_N.htm.

Wells, K. (2013). Self-regulation technique helps students focus in class: Teachers try new approach to improving

students’ behaviour. CBC News Posted: Nov 30, 2013 Retrieved 2014 from http://www.cbc.ca/news/canada/selfregulation-

technique-helps-students-focus-in-class-1.2440688

Williams, L. E., Bargh, J. A., Nocera, C. C., & Gray, J. R. (2009). The unconscious regulation of emotion:

Nonconscious reappraisal goals modulate emotional reactivity. Emotion, 9(6), 847–854.

Image Attributions:

Figure 11.1: Sully Sullenberger by Ingrid Taylar (http://www.flickr.com/photos/taylar/435061088) used under

CC BY 2.0 license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0/); Plane crash into Hudson River by Greg L.,

(http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Plane_crash_into_Hudson_Rivercroped.jpg) used under CC BY 2.0

license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0/deed.en).

433 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

11.1 The Experience of Emotion

Learning Objectives

1. Explain the biological experience of emotion.

2. Summarize the psychological theories of emotion.

3. Give examples of the ways that emotion is communicated.

The most fundamental emotions, known as the basic emotions, are those of anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness,

and surprise. The basic emotions have a long history in human evolution, and they have developed in large part to

help us make rapid judgments about stimuli and to quickly guide appropriate behaviour (LeDoux, 2000). The basic

emotions are determined in large part by one of the oldest parts of our brain, the limbic system, including the

amygdala, the hypothalamus, and the thalamus. Because they are primarily evolutionarily determined, the basic

emotions are experienced and displayed in much the same way across cultures (Ekman, 1992; Elfenbein & Ambady,

2002; Fridland, Ekman, & Oster, 1987), and people are quite accurate at judging the facial expressions of people

from different cultures. View “Video Clip: The Basic Emotions,” to see a demonstration of the basic emotions.

Watch: “Recognize Basic Emotions” [YouTube]: http://www.youtube.com/

watch?v=haW6E7qsW2c

Not all of our emotions come from the old parts of our brain; we also interpret our

experiences to create a more complex array of emotional experiences. For instance, the

amygdala may sense fear when it senses that the body is falling, but that fear may be

interpreted completely differently (perhaps even as excitement) when we are falling on

a roller-coaster ride than when we are falling from the sky in an airplane that has lost

power. The cognitive interpretations that accompany emotions —known as cognitive

appraisal —allow us to experience a much larger and more complex set of secondary

emotions, as shown in Figure 11.2, “The Secondary Emotions.” Although they are in large part cognitive, our

experiences of the secondary emotions are determined in part by arousal (on the vertical axis of Figure 11.2, “The

Secondary Emotions”) and in part by their valence — that is, whether they are pleasant or unpleasant feelings (on

the horizontal axis of Figure 11.2, “The Secondary Emotions”),

When you succeed in reaching an important goal, you might spend some time enjoying your secondary emotions,

perhaps the experience of joy, satisfaction, and contentment. But when your close friend wins a prize that you

thought you had deserved, you might also experience a variety of secondary emotions (in this case, the negative

ones) — for instance, feeling angry, sad, resentful, and ashamed. You might mull over the event for weeks or even

months, experiencing these negative emotions each time you think about it (Martin & Tesser, 2006).

The distinction between the primary and the secondary emotions is paralleled by two brain pathways: a fast pathway

and a slow pathway (Damasio, 2000; LeDoux, 2000; Ochsner, Bunge, Gross, & Gabrieli, 2002). The thalamus acts

as the major gatekeeper in this process (Figure 11.3, “Slow and Fast Emotional Pathways”). Our response to the

434

Figure 11.2 The Secondary Emotions. The secondary emotions are those that have a major

cognitive component. They are determined by both their level of arousal (mild to intense) and

their valence (pleasant to unpleasant). [Long Description]

basic emotion of fear, for instance, is primarily determined by the fast pathway through the limbic system. When a

car pulls out in front of us on the highway, the thalamus activates and sends an immediate message to the amygdala.

We quickly move our foot to the brake pedal. Secondary emotions are more determined by the slow pathway

through the frontal lobes in the cortex. When we stew in jealousy over the loss of a partner to a rival or recollect our

win in the big tennis match, the process is more complex. Information moves from the thalamus to the frontal lobes

for cognitive analysis and integration, and then from there to the amygdala. We experience the arousal of emotion,

but it is accompanied by a more complex cognitive appraisal, producing more refined emotions and behavioural

responses.

Although emotions might seem to you to be more frivolous or less important in comparison to our more rational

cognitive processes, both emotions and cognitions can help us make effective decisions. In some cases we take

action after rationally processing the costs and benefits of different choices, but in other cases we rely on our

emotions. Emotions become particularly important in guiding decisions when the alternatives between many

complex and conflicting alternatives present us with a high degree of uncertainty and ambiguity, making a complete

cognitive analysis difficult. In these cases we often rely on our emotions to make decisions, and these decisions may

in many cases be more accurate than those produced by cognitive processing (Damasio, 1994; Dijksterhuis, Bos,

Nordgren, & van Baaren, 2006; Nordgren & Dijksterhuis, 2009; Wilson & Schooler, 1991).

The Cannon-Bard and James-Lange Theories of Emotion

Recall for a moment a situation in which you have experienced an intense emotional response. Perhaps you woke

435 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Figure 11.3 Slow and Fast Emotional Pathways. There are two emotional pathways in the brain

(one slow and one fast), both of which are controlled by the thalamus.

up in the middle of the night in a panic because you heard a noise that made you think that someone had broken into

your house or apartment. Or maybe you were calmly cruising down a street in your neighbourhood when another

car suddenly pulled out in front of you, forcing you to slam on your brakes to avoid an accident. I’m sure that you

remember that your emotional reaction was in large part physical. Perhaps you remember being flushed, your heart

pounding, feeling sick to your stomach, or having trouble breathing. You were experiencing the physiological part

of emotion — arousal — and I’m sure you have had similar feelings in other situations, perhaps when you were in

love, angry, embarrassed, frustrated, or very sad.

If you think back to a strong emotional experience, you might wonder about the order of the events that occurred.

Certainly you experienced arousal, but did the arousal come before, after, or along with the experience of the

emotion? Psychologists have proposed three different theories of emotion, which differ in terms of the hypothesized

role of arousal in emotion (Figure 11.4, “Three Theories of Emotion”).

If your experiences are like mine, as you reflected on the arousal that you have experienced in strong emotional

situations, you probably thought something like, “I was afraid and my heart started beating like crazy.” At least

some psychologists agree with this interpretation. According to the theory of emotion proposed by Walter Cannon

and Philip Bard, the experience of the emotion (in this case, “I’m afraid”) occurs alongside the experience of the

arousal (“my heart is beating fast”). According to the Cannon-Bard theory of emotion, the experience of an

emotion is accompanied by physiological arousal. Thus, according to this model of emotion, as we become aware

of danger, our heart rate also increases.

Although the idea that the experience of an emotion occurs alongside the accompanying arousal seems intuitive

to our everyday experiences, the psychologists William James and Carl Lange had another idea about the role of

arousal. According to the James-Lange theory of emotion, our experience of an emotion is the result of the arousal

11.1 THE EXPERIENCE OF EMOTION • 436

Figure 11.4 Three Theories of Emotion. The Cannon-Bard theory proposes that emotions and

arousal occur at the same time. The James-Lange theory proposes the emotion is the result of

arousal. Schachter and Singer’s two-factor model proposes that arousal and cognition combine to

create emotion.

that we experience. This approach proposes that the arousal and the emotion are not independent, but rather that

the emotion depends on the arousal. The fear does not occur along with the racing heart but occurs because of the

racing heart. As William James put it, “We feel sorry because we cry, angry because we strike, afraid because we

tremble” (James, 1884, p. 190). A fundamental aspect of the James-Lange theory is that different patterns of arousal

may create different emotional experiences.

There is research evidence to support each of these theories. The operation of the fast emotional pathway (Figure

11.4, “Slow and Fast Emotional Pathways”) supports the idea that arousal and emotions occur together. The

emotional circuits in the limbic system are activated when an emotional stimulus is experienced, and these circuits

quickly create corresponding physical reactions (LeDoux, 2000). The process happens so quickly that it may feel to

us as if emotion is simultaneous with our physical arousal.

On the other hand, and as predicted by the James-Lange theory, our experiences of emotion are weaker without

arousal. Patients who have spinal injuries that reduce their experience of arousal also report decreases in emotional

responses (Hohmann, 1966). There is also at least some support for the idea that different emotions are produced by

different patterns of arousal. People who view fearful faces show more amygdala activation than those who watch

angry or joyful faces (Whalen et al., 2001; Witvliet & Vrana, 1995), we experience a red face and flushing when

we are embarrassed but not when we experience other emotions (Leary, Britt, Cutlip, & Templeton, 1992), and

437 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

different hormones are released when we experience compassion than when we experience other emotions (Oatley,

Keltner, & Jenkins, 2006).

The Two-Factor Theory of Emotion

Whereas the James-Lange theory proposes that each emotion has a different pattern of arousal, the two-factor theory

of emotion takes the opposite approach, arguing that the arousal that we experience is basically the same in every

emotion, and that all emotions (including the basic emotions) are differentiated only by our cognitive appraisal of

the source of the arousal. The two-factor theory of emotion asserts that the experience of emotion is determined

by the intensity of the arousal we are experiencing, but that the cognitive appraisal of the situation determines what

the emotion will be. Because both arousal and appraisal are necessary, we can say that emotions have two factors:

an arousal factor and a cognitive factor (Schachter & Singer, 1962):

emotion = arousal + cognition

In some cases it may be difficult for a person who is experiencing a high level of arousal to accurately determine

which emotion he or she is experiencing. That is, the person may be certain that he or she is feeling arousal, but the

meaning of the arousal (the cognitive factor) may be less clear. Some romantic relationships, for instance, have a

very high level of arousal, and the partners alternatively experience extreme highs and lows in the relationship. One

day they are madly in love with each other and the next they are in a huge fight. In situations that are accompanied

by high arousal, people may be unsure what emotion they are experiencing. In the high arousal relationship, for

instance, the partners may be uncertain whether the emotion they are feeling is love, hate, or both at the same time.

The tendency for people to incorrectly label the source of the arousal that they are experiencing is known as the

misattribution of arousal.

Figure 11.5 Capilano Suspension Bridge. Arousal caused by the height of this bridge was

misattributed as attraction by the men who were interviewed by an attractive woman as they

crossed it.

In one interesting field study by Dutton and Aron (1974), an attractive young woman approached individual young

men as they crossed a wobbly, long suspension walkway hanging more than 200 feet above a river in British

11.1 THE EXPERIENCE OF EMOTION • 438

Columbia (Figure 11.5, “Capilano Suspension Bridge”). The woman asked each man to help her fill out a class

questionnaire. When he had finished, she wrote her name and phone number on a piece of paper, and invited him

to call if he wanted to hear more about the project. More than half of the men who had been interviewed on the

bridge later called the woman. In contrast, men approached by the same woman on a low, solid bridge, or who

were interviewed on the suspension bridge by men, called significantly less frequently. The idea of misattribution of

arousal can explain this result — the men were feeling arousal from the height of the bridge, but they misattributed

it as romantic or sexual attraction to the woman, making them more likely to call her.

Research Focus: Misattributing Arousal

If you think a bit about your own experiences of different emotions, and if you consider the equation that

suggests that emotions are represented by both arousal and cognition, you might start to wonder how much

was determined by each. That is, do we know what emotion we are experiencing by monitoring our feelings

(arousal) or by monitoring our thoughts (cognition)? The bridge study you just read about might begin to

provide you with an answer: The men seemed to be more influenced by their perceptions of how they should

be feeling (their cognition) rather than by how they actually were feeling (their arousal).

Stanley Schachter and Jerome Singer (1962) directly tested this prediction of the two-factor theory of

emotion in a well-known experiment. Schachter and Singer believed that the cognitive part of the emotion

was critical—in fact, they believed that the arousal that we experience could be interpreted as any emotion,

provided we had the right label for it. Thus they hypothesized that if an individual is experiencing arousal for

which there is no immediate explanation, that individual will “label” this state in terms of the cognitions that

are created in his or her environment. On the other hand, they argued that people who already have a clear

label for their arousal would have no need to search for a relevant label, and therefore should not experience

an emotion.

In the research, male participants were told that they would be participating in a study on the effects of a

new drug, called suproxin, on vision. On the basis of this cover story, the men were injected with a shot of

the neurotransmitter epinephrine, a drug that normally creates feelings of tremors, flushing, and accelerated

breathing in people. The idea was to give all the participants the experience of arousal.

Then, according to random assignment to conditions, the men were told that the drug would make them

feel certain ways. The men in the epinephrine informed condition were told the truth about the effects of

the drug — that they would likely experience tremors, their hands would start to shake, their hearts would

start to pound, and their faces might get warm and flushed. The participants in the epinephrine-uninformed

condition, however, were told something untrue — that their feet would feel numb, they would have an

itching sensation over parts of their body, and they might get a slight headache. The idea was to make some

of the men think that the arousal they were experiencing was caused by the drug (the informed condition),

whereas others would be unsure where the arousal came from (the uninformed condition).

Then the men were left alone with a confederate who they thought had received the same injection. While

they were waiting for the experiment (which was supposedly about vision) to begin, the confederate behaved

in a wild and crazy manner (Schachter and Singer called it a “euphoric” manner). He wadded up spitballs,

flew paper airplanes, and played with a hula-hoop. He kept trying to get the participant to join in with his

games. Then right before the vision experiment was to begin, the participants were asked to indicate their

current emotional states on a number of scales. One of the emotions they were asked about was euphoria.

If you are following the story, you will realize what was expected: The men who had a label for their arousal

439 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

(the informed group) would not be experiencing much emotion because they already had a label available

for their arousal. The men in the misinformed group, on the other hand, were expected to be unsure about the

source of the arousal. They needed to find an explanation for their arousal, and the confederate provided one.

As you can see in Figure 11.6 ,”Results from Schachter and Singer, 1962″ (left side), this is just what they

found. The participants in the misinformed condition were more likely to experience euphoria (as measured

by their behavioural responses with the confederate) than were those in the informed condition.

Then Schachter and Singer conducted another part of the study, using new participants. Everything was

exactly the same except for the behaviour of the confederate. Rather than being euphoric, he acted angry. He

complained about having to complete the questionnaire he had been asked to do, indicating that the questions

were stupid and too personal. He ended up tearing up the questionnaire that he was working on, yelling, “I

don’t have to tell them that!” Then he grabbed his books and stormed out of the room.

What do you think happened in this condition? The answer is the same thing: the misinformed participants

experienced more anger (again as measured by the participant’s behaviours during the waiting period) than

did the informed participants. (Figure 11.6, “Results from Schachter and Singer, 1962”, right side). The idea

is that because cognitions are such strong determinants of emotional states, the same state of physiological

arousal could be labelled in many different ways, depending entirely on the label provided by the social

situation. As Schachter and Singer put it: “Given a state of physiological arousal for which an individual

has no immediate explanation, he will ‘label’ this state and describe his feelings in terms of the cognitions

available to him” (Schachter & Singer, 1962, p. 381).

Figure 11.6 Results from Schachter and Singer, 1962. Results of the study by Schachter and Singer

(1962) support the two-factor theory of emotion. The participants who did not have a clear label

for their arousal took on the emotion of the confederate.

Because it assumes that arousal is constant across emotions, the two-factor theory also predicts that emotions may

transfer or spill over from one highly arousing event to another. My university basketball team recently won a

basketball championship, but after the final victory some students rioted in the streets near the campus, lighting

fires and burning cars. This seems to be a very strange reaction to such a positive outcome for the university

and the students, but it can be explained through the spillover of the arousal caused by happiness to destructive

behaviours. The principle of excitation transfer refers to the phenomenon that occurs when people who are already

experiencing arousal from one event tend to also experience unrelated emotions more strongly.

In sum, each of the three theories of emotion has something to support it. In terms of Cannon-Bard, emotions and

11.1 THE EXPERIENCE OF EMOTION • 440

arousal generally are subjectively experienced together, and the spread is very fast. In support of the James-Lange

theory, there is at least some evidence that arousal is necessary for the experience of emotion, and that the patterns

of arousal are different for different emotions. And in line with the two-factor model, there is also evidence that we

may interpret the same patterns of arousal differently in different situations.

Communicating Emotion

In addition to experiencing emotions internally, we also express our emotions to others, and we learn about

the emotions of others by observing them. This communication process has evolved over time and is highly

adaptive. One way that we perceive the emotions of others is through their nonverbal communication, that is,

communication, primarily of liking or disliking, that does not involve words (Ambady & Weisbuch, 2010; Andersen,

2007). Nonverbal communication includes our tone of voice, gait, posture, touch, and facial expressions, and we can

often accurately detect the emotions that other people are experiencing through these channels. Table 11.1, “Some

Common Nonverbal Communicators,” shows some of the important nonverbal behaviours that we use to express

emotion and some other information (particularly liking or disliking, and dominance or submission).

Table 11.1 Some Common Nonverbal Communicators.

[Skip Table]

Nonverbal cue Description Examples

Proxemics Rules about the appropriate use of

personal space Standing nearer to someone can express liking or dominance.

Body

appearance

Expressions based on alterations to

our body

Body building, breast augmentation, weight loss, piercings, and

tattoos are often used to appear more attractive to others.

Body

positioning and

movement

Expressions based on how our body

appears

A more “open” body position can denote liking; a faster walking

speed can communicate dominance.

Gestures Behaviours and signs made with our

hands or faces

The peace sign communicates liking; the “finger” communicates

disrespect.

Facial

expressions

The variety of emotions that we

express, or attempt to hide, through

our face

Smiling or frowning and staring or avoiding looking at the other

can express liking or disliking, as well as dominance or

submission.

Paralanguage Clues to identity or emotions

contained in our voices

Pronunciation, accents, and dialect can be used to communicate

identity and liking.

Just as there is no universal spoken language, there is no universal nonverbal language. For instance, in Canada we

express disrespect by showing the middle finger (the finger or the bird). But in Britain, Ireland, Australia, and New

Zealand, the V sign (made with back of the hand facing the recipient) serves a similar purpose. In countries where

Spanish, Portuguese, or French are spoken, a gesture in which a fist is raised and the arm is slapped on the bicep is

equivalent to the finger, and in Russia, Indonesia, Turkey, and China a sign in which the hand and fingers are curled

and the thumb is thrust between the middle and index fingers is used for the same purpose.

The most important communicator of emotion is the face. The face contains 43 different muscles that allow it to

make more than 10,000 unique configurations and to express a wide variety of emotions. For example, happiness is

expressed by smiles, which are created by two of the major muscles surrounding the mouth and the eyes, and anger

is created by lowered brows and firmly pressed lips.

441 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

In addition to helping us express our emotions, the face also helps us feel emotion. The facial feedback hypothesis

proposes that the movement of our facial muscles can trigger corresponding emotions. Fritz Strack and his

colleagues (1988) asked their research participants to hold a pen in their teeth (mimicking the facial action of a

smile) or between their lips (similar to a frown), and then had them rate the funniness of a cartoon. They found that

the cartoons were rated as more amusing when the pen was held in the smiling position—the subjective experience

of emotion was intensified by the action of the facial muscles.

These results, and others like them, show that our behaviours, including our facial expressions, both influence and

are influenced by our affect. We may smile because we are happy, but we are also happy because we are smiling.

And we may stand up straight because we are proud, but we are proud because we are standing up straight (Stepper

& Strack, 1993).

Key Takeaways

• Emotions are the normally adaptive mental and physiological feeling states that direct our

attention and guide our behaviour.

• Emotional states are accompanied by arousal, our experiences of the bodily responses created by

the sympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system.

• Motivations are forces that guide behaviour. They can be biological, such as hunger and thirst;

personal, such as the motivation for achievement; or social, such as the motivation for acceptance

and belonging.

• The most fundamental emotions, known as the basic emotions, are those of anger, disgust, fear,

happiness, sadness, and surprise.

• Cognitive appraisal also allows us to experience a variety of secondary emotions.

• According to the Cannon-Bard theory of emotion, the experience of an emotion is accompanied

by physiological arousal.

• According to the James-Lange theory of emotion, our experience of an emotion is the result of the

arousal that we experience.

• According to the two-factor theory of emotion, the experience of emotion is determined by the

intensity of the arousal we are experiencing, and the cognitive appraisal of the situation

determines what the emotion will be.

• When people incorrectly label the source of the arousal that they are experiencing, we say that

they have misattributed their arousal.

• We express our emotions to others through nonverbal behaviours, and we learn about the

emotions of others by observing them.

11.1 THE EXPERIENCE OF EMOTION • 442

Exercises and Critical Thinking

1. Consider the three theories of emotion that we have discussed and provide an example of a

situation in which a person might experience each of the three proposed patterns of arousal and

emotion.

2. Describe a time when you used nonverbal behaviours to express your emotions or to detect the

emotions of others. What specific nonverbal techniques did you use to communicate?

References

Ambady, N., & Weisbuch, M. (2010). Nonverbal behavior. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey

(Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., Vol. 1, pp. 464–497). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Andersen, P. (2007). Nonverbal communication: Forms and functions (2nd ed.). Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press.

Damasio, A. (2000). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York,

NY: Mariner Books.

Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York, NY: Grosset/Putnam.

Dijksterhuis, A., Bos, M. W., Nordgren, L. F., & van Baaren, R. B. (2006). On making the right choice: The

deliberation-without-attention effect. Science, 311(5763), 1005–1007.

Dutton, D., & Aron, A. (1974). Some evidence for heightened sexual attraction under conditions of high

anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 510–517.

Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99(3), 550–553.

Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A metaanalysis.

Psychological Bulletin, 128, 203–23.

Fridlund, A. J., Ekman, P., & Oster, H. (1987). Facial expressions of emotion. In A. Siegman & S. Feldstein

(Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication (2nd ed., pp. 143–223). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Hohmann, G. W. (1966). Some effects of spinal cord lesions on experienced emotional feelings. Psychophysiology,

3(2), 143–156.

James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 9(34), 188–205.

Leary, M. R., Britt, T. W., Cutlip, W. D., & Templeton, J. L. (1992). Social blushing. Psychological Bulletin,

112(3), 446–460.

LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155–184.

Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (2006). Extending the goal progress theory of rumination: Goal reevaluation and growth.

443 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

In L. J. Sanna & E. C. Chang (Eds.), Judgments over time: The interplay of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (pp.

145–162). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Nordgren, L. F., & Dijksterhuis, A. P. (2009). The devil is in the deliberation: Thinking too much reduces preference

consistency. Journal of Consumer Research, 36(1), 39–46.

Oatley, K., Keltner, D., & Jenkins, J. M. (2006). Understanding emotions (2nd ed.). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2002). Rethinking feelings: An fMRI study of the

cognitive regulation of emotion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(8), 1215–1229.

Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.

Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional

state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 64(2), 211–220.

Strack, F., Martin, L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A

nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 768–777.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768

Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., Wright, C. I., & Rauch, S. L. (2001). A functional MRI

study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. Emotion, 1(1), 70–83;

Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences

and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 181–192.

Witvliet, C. V., & Vrana, S. R. (1995). Psychophysiological responses as indices of affective

dimensions. Psychophysiology, 32(5), 436–443.

Image Attributions

Figure 11.2: Adapted from Russell, 1980.

Figure 11.5: Capilano suspension bridge by Goobiebilly (http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/

File:Capilano_suspension_bridge_-g.jpg) used under CC-BY 2.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0/

deed.en).

Figure 11.6: Adapted from Schachter & Singer, 1962.

11.1 THE EXPERIENCE OF EMOTION • 444

Long Descriptions

Figure 11.2 long description: The Secondary Emotions

Level of Arousal Unpleasant Pleasant

Mild

• Miserable

• Sad

• Depressed

• Gloomy

• Bored

• Droopy

• Content

• Satisfied

• At ease

• Serene

• Calm

• Relaxed

• Sleepy

• Tired

Intense

• Alarmed

• Afraid

• Angry

• Intense

• Annoyed

• Frustrated

• Distressed

• Astonished

• Excited

• Amused

• Happy

• Delighted

• Glad Pleased

[Return to Figure 11.2]

445 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

11.2 Stress: The Unseen Killer

Learning Objectives

1. Define stress and review the body’s physiological responses to it.

2. Summarize the negative health consequences of prolonged stress.

3. Explain the differences in how people respond to stress.

4. Review the methods that are successful in coping with stress.

Emotions matter because they influence our behaviour. And there is no emotional experience that has a more

powerful influence on us than stress. Stress refers to the physiological responses that occur when an organism fails

to respond appropriately to emotional or physical threats (Selye, 1956). Extreme negative events, such as being

the victim of a terrorist attack, a natural disaster, or a violent crime, may produce an extreme form of stress known

as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), a medical syndrome that includes symptoms of anxiety, sleeplessness,

nightmares, and social withdrawal. PTSD is frequently experienced by victims or witnesses of violence or abuse,

natural disasters, major accidents, or war.

When it is extreme or prolonged, stress can create substantial health problems. A study out of the University of

British Columbia found that emergency personnel such as doctors, nurses, paramedics, and firefighters experience

post-traumatic stress at twice the rate of the average population. In Canada, it is estimated that up to 10% of war

zone veterans — including war service veterans and peacekeeping forces — will experience post-traumatic stress

disorder (CMHA, 2014). People in New York City who lived nearer to the site of the 9/11 terrorist attacks reported

experiencing more stress in the year following it than those who lived farther away (Pulcino et al., 2003). But stress

is not unique to the experience of extremely traumatic events. It can also occur, and have a variety of negative

outcomes, in our everyday lives.

The Negative Effects of Stress

The physiologist Hans Selye (1907-1982) studied stress by examining how rats responded to being exposed to

stressors such as extreme cold, infection, shock, or excessive exercise (Selye, 1936, 1974, 1982). Selye found that

regardless of the source of the stress, the rats experienced the same series of physiological changes as they suffered

the prolonged stress. Selye created the term general adaptation syndrome to refer to the three distinct phases of

physiological change that occur in response to long-term stress: alarm, resistance, and exhaustion (Figure 11.7,

“General Adaptation Syndrome”).

The experience of stress creates both an increase in general arousal in the sympathetic division of the autonomic

nervous system (ANS), as well as another, even more complex, system of physiological changes through the HPA

axis. The HPA axis is a physiological response to stress involving interactions among the (H) hypothalamus, the (P)

pituitary, and the (A) adrenal glands (Figure 11.8, “HPA Axis”). The HPA response begins when the hypothalamus

secretes releasing hormones that direct the pituitary gland to release the hormone ACTH. ACTH then directs the

446

Figure 11.7 General Adaptation Syndrome. Hans Selye’s research on the general adaptation

syndrome documented the stages of prolonged exposure to stress. [Long Description]

adrenal glands to secrete more hormones, including epinephrine, norepinephrine, and cortisol, a stress hormone that

releases sugars into the blood, helping preparing the body to respond to threat (Rodrigues, LeDoux, & Sapolsky,

2009).

The initial arousal that accompanies stress is normally quite adaptive because it helps us respond to potentially

dangerous events. The experience of prolonged stress, however, has a direct negative influence on our physical

health, because at the same time that stress increases activity in the sympathetic division of the ANS, it also

suppresses activity in the parasympathetic division of the ANS. When stress is long term, the HPA axis remains

active and the adrenals continue to produce cortisol. This increased cortisol production exhausts the stress

mechanism, leading to fatigue and depression.

The HPA reactions to persistent stress lead to a weakening of the immune system, making us more susceptible to

a variety of health problems including colds and other diseases (Cohen & Herbert, 1996; Faulkner & Smith, 2009;

Miller, Chen, & Cole, 2009; Uchino, Smith, Holt-Lunstad, Campo, & Reblin, 2007). Stress also damages our DNA,

447 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Figure 11.8 HPA Axis. Stress activates the HPA axis. The result is the secretion of epinephrine,

norepinephrine, and cortisol.

making us less likely to be able to repair wounds and respond to the genetic mutations that cause disease (Epel et

al., 2006). As a result, wounds heal more slowly when we are under stress, and we are more likely to get cancer

(Kiecolt-Glaser, McGuire, Robles, & Glaser, 2002; Wells, 2006).

Chronic stress is also a major contributor to heart disease. Although heart disease is caused in part by genetic

factors, as well as high blood pressure, high cholesterol, and cigarette smoking, it is also caused by stress (Krantz

& McCeney, 2002). Long-term stress creates two opposite effects on the coronary system. Stress increases cardiac

output (i.e., the heart pumps more blood) at the same time that it reduces the ability of the blood vessels to conduct

blood through the arteries, as the increase in levels of cortisol leads to a buildup of plaque on artery walls (Dekker

et al., 2008). The combination of increased blood flow and arterial constriction leads to increased blood pressure

(hypertension), which can damage the heart muscle, leading to heart attack and death.

Stressors in Our Everyday Lives

The stressors for Selye’s rats included electric shock and exposure to cold. Although these are probably not on your

top-10 list of most common stressors, the stress that you experience in your everyday life can also be taxing. Thomas

Holmes and Richard Rahe (1967) developed a measure of some everyday life events that might lead to stress, and

you can assess your own likely stress level by completing the measure in Table 11.2, “The Holmes and Rahe Stress

Scale.” You might want to pay particular attention to this score, because it can predict the likelihood that you will

get sick. Rahe and colleagues examined the medical records of over 5,000 patients to determine whether stressful

events might cause illnesses. Patients were asked to tally a list of 43 life events based on a relative score. A positive

11.2 STRESS: THE UNSEEN KILLER • 448

correlation of 0.118 was found between their life events and their illnesses resulting in the Social Readjustment

Rating Scale (SRRS) (Rahe, Mahan, Arthur, & Gunderson, 1970). Rahe and colleagues (2000) went on to update

and revalidate the scale. Reliability testing, using Cronbach alpha correlations, was performed utilizing a sample of

1,772 individuals. The SRRS is commonly used today.

449 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Table 11.2 The Holmes and Rahe Stress Scale.

[Skip Table]

Life event Score

Death of spouse 100

Divorce 73

Marital separation from mate 65

Detention in jail, other institution 63

Death of a close family member 63

Major personal injury or illness 53

Marriage 50

Fired from work 47

Marital reconciliation 45

Retirement 45

Major change in the health or behaviour of a family member 44

Pregnancy 40

Sexual difficulties 39

Gaining a new family member (e.g., through birth, adoption, oldster moving) 39

Major business readjustment (e.g., merger, reorganization, bankruptcy) 39

Major change in financial status 38

Death of close friend 37

Change to different line of work 36

Major change in the number of arguments with spouse 35

Taking out a mortgage or loan for a major purchase 31

Foreclosure on a mortgage or loan 30

Major change in responsibilities at work 29

Son or daughter leaving home (e.g., marriage, attending university) 29

Trouble with in-laws 29

Outstanding personal achievement 28

Spouse beginning or ceasing to work outside the home 26

Beginning or ceasing formal schooling 26

Major change in living conditions 25

Revision of personal habits (dress, manners, associations, etc.) 24

Trouble with boss 23

11.2 STRESS: THE UNSEEN KILLER • 450

[Skip Table]

Life event Score

Major change in working hours or conditions 20

Change in residence 20

Change to a new school 20

Major change in usual type and/or amount of recreation 19

Major change in church activities (a lot more or less than usual) 19

Major change in social activities (clubs, dancing, movies, visiting) 18

Taking out a mortgage or loan for a lesser purchase (e.g., for a car, television, freezer) 17

Major change in sleeping habits 16

Major change in the number of family get-togethers 15

Major change in eating habits 15

Vacation 13

Christmas season 12

Minor violations of the law (e.g., traffic tickets) 11

Total ______

You can calculate your score on this scale by adding the total points across each of the events that you have

experienced over the past year. Then use Table 11.3, “Interpretation of Holmes and Rahe Stress Scale” to determine

your likelihood of getting ill.

Table 11.3 Interpretation of Holmes and Rahe Stress Scale.

[Skip Table]

Number of life-change units Chance of developing a stress-related illness (%)

Less than 150 30

150–299 50

More than 300 80

Although some of the items on the Holmes and Rahe scale are more major, you can see that even minor stressors

add to the total score. Our everyday interactions with the environment that are essentially negative, known as daily

hassles, can also create stress as well as poorer health outcomes (Hutchinson & Williams, 2007). Events that may

seem rather trivial altogether, such as misplacing our keys, having to reboot our computer because it has frozen,

being late for an assignment, or getting cut off by another car in rush-hour traffic, can produce stress (Fiksenbaum,

Greenglass, & Eaton, 2006). Glaser (1985) found that medical students who were tested during, rather than several

weeks before, their school examination periods showed lower immune system functioning. Other research has

found that even more minor stressors, such as having to do math problems during an experimental session, can

compromise the immune system (Cacioppo et al., 1998).

451 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Responses to Stress

Not all people experience and respond to stress in the same way, and these differences can be important. The

cardiologists Meyer Friedman and R. H. Rosenman (1974) were among the first to study the link between stress

and heart disease. In their research they noticed that even though the partners in married couples often had similar

lifestyles, diet, and exercise patterns, the husbands nevertheless generally had more heart disease than the wives did.

As they tried to explain the difference, they focused on the personality characteristics of the partners, finding that

the husbands were more likely than the wives to respond to stressors with negative emotions and hostility.

Recent research has shown that the strongest predictor of a physiological stress response from daily hassles is

the amount of negative emotion that they evoke. People who experience strong negative emotions as a result of

everyday hassles, and who respond to stress with hostility, experience more negative health outcomes than do

those who react in a less negative way (McIntyre, Korn, & Matsuo, 2008; Suls & Bunde, 2005). Williams and his

colleagues (2001) found that people who scored high on measures of anger were three times more likely to suffer

from heart attacks in comparison to those who scored lower on anger.

On average, men are more likely than women are to respond to stress by activating the fight-or-flight response,

which is an emotional and behavioural reaction to stress that increases the readiness for action. The arousal that

men experience when they are stressed leads them to either go on the attack, in an aggressive or revenging way, or

else retreat as quickly as they can to safety from the stressor. The fight-or-flight response allows men to control the

source of the stress if they think they can do so, or if that is not possible, it allows them to save face by leaving the

situation. The fight-or-flight response is triggered in men by the activation of the HPA axis.

Women, on the other hand, are less likely to take a fight-or-flight response to stress. Rather, they are more likely to

take a tend-and-befriend response (Taylor et al., 2000). The tend-and-befriend response is a behavioural reaction

to stress that involves activities designed to create social networks that provide protection from threats. This

approach is also self-protective because it allows the individual to talk to others about her concerns, as well as

to exchange resources, such as child care. The tend-and-befriend response is triggered in women by the release

of the hormone oxytocin, which promotes affiliation. Overall, the tend-and-befriend response is healthier than the

flight-or-flight response because it does not produce the elevated levels of arousal related to the HPA, including the

negative results that accompany increased levels of cortisol. This may help explain why women, on average, have

less heart disease and live longer than men.

Managing Stress

No matter how healthy and happy we are in our everyday lives, there are going to be times when we experience

stress. But we do not need to throw up our hands in despair when things go wrong; rather, we can use our personal

and social resources to help us.

Perhaps the most common approach to dealing with negative affect is to attempt to suppress, avoid, or deny it.

You probably know people who seem to be stressed, depressed, or anxious, but they cannot or will not see it in

themselves. Perhaps you tried to talk to them about it, to get them to open up to you, but were rebuffed. They seem

to act as if there is no problem at all, simply moving on with life without admitting or even trying to deal with the

negative feelings. Or perhaps you have even taken a similar approach yourself. Have you ever had an important test

to study for or an important job interview coming up, and rather than planning and preparing for it, you simply tried

to put it out of your mind entirely?

Research has found that ignoring stress is not a good approach for coping with it. For one, ignoring our problems

does not make them go away. If we experience so much stress that we get sick, these events will be detrimental

11.2 STRESS: THE UNSEEN KILLER • 452

to our life even if we do not or cannot admit that they are occurring. Suppressing our negative emotions is also

not a very good option, at least in the long run, because it tends to fail (Gross & Levenson, 1997). For one, if we

know that we have that big exam coming up, we have to focus on the exam itself to suppress it. We can’t really

suppress or deny our thoughts, because we actually have to recall and face the event to make the attempt to not

think about it. Doing so takes effort, and we get tired when we try to do it. Furthermore, we may continually worry

that our attempts to suppress will fail. Suppressing our emotions might work out for a short while, but when we run

out of energy the negative emotions may shoot back up into consciousness, causing us to reexperience the negative

feelings that we had been trying to avoid.

Daniel Wegner and his colleagues (Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987) directly tested whether people

would be able to effectively suppress a simple thought. He asked them to not think about a white bear for five

minutes but to ring a bell in case they did. (Try it yourself; can you do it?) However, participants were unable

to suppress the thought as instructed. The white bear kept popping into mind, even when the participants were

instructed to avoid thinking about it. You might have had this experience when you were dieting or trying to study

rather than party; the chocolate bar in the kitchen cabinet and the fun time you were missing at the party kept

popping into mind, disrupting you.

Suppressing our negative thoughts does not work, and there is evidence that the opposite is true: when we are faced

with troubles, it is healthy to let out the negative thoughts and feelings by expressing them, either to ourselves

or to others. James Pennebaker and his colleagues (Pennebaker, Colder, & Sharp, 1990; Watson & Pennebaker,

1989) have conducted many correlational and experimental studies that demonstrate the advantages to our mental

and physical health of opening up versus suppressing our feelings. This research team has found that simply talking

about or writing about our emotions or our reactions to negative events provides substantial health benefits. For

instance, Pennebaker and Beall (1986) randomly assigned students to write about either the most traumatic and

stressful event of their lives or trivial topics. Although the students who wrote about the traumas had higher blood

pressure and more negative moods immediately after they wrote their essays, they were also less likely to visit

the student health centre for illnesses during the following six months. Other research studied individuals whose

spouses had died in the previous year, finding that the more they talked about the death with others, the less likely

they were to become ill during the subsequent year. Daily writing about one’s emotional states has also been found

to increase immune system functioning (Petrie, Fontanilla, Thomas, Booth, & Pennebaker, 2004).

Opening up probably helps in various ways. For one, expressing our problems to others allows us to gain

information, and possibly support, from them (remember the tend-and-befriend response that is so effectively used

to reduce stress by women). Writing or thinking about one’s experiences also seems to help people make sense of

these events and may give them a feeling of control over their lives (Pennebaker & Stone, 2004).

It is easier to respond to stress if we can interpret it in more positive ways. Kelsey et al. (1999) found that some

people interpret stress as a challenge (something that they feel that they can, with effort, deal with), whereas others

see the same stress as a threat (something that is negative and to be feared). People who viewed stress as a challenge

had fewer physiological stress responses than those who viewed it as a threat — they were able to frame and react

to stress in more positive ways.

Emotion Regulation

Emotional responses such as the stress reaction are useful in warning us about potential danger and in mobilizing our

response to it, so it is a good thing that we have them. However, we also need to learn how to control our emotions,

to prevent them from letting our behaviour get out of control. The ability to successfully control our emotions is

known as emotion regulation.

453 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Emotion regulation has some important positive outcomes. Consider, for instance, research by Walter Mischel and

his colleagues. In their studies, they had four- and five-year-old children sit at a table in front of a yummy snack,

such as a chocolate chip cookie or a marshmallow. The children were told that they could eat the snack right away

if they wanted. However, they were also told that if they could wait for just a couple of minutes, they’d be able to

have two snacks — both the one in front of them and another just like it. However, if they ate the one that was in

front of them before the time was up, they would not get a second.

Mischel found that some children were able to override the impulse to seek immediate gratification to obtain

a greater reward at a later time. Other children, of course, were not; they just ate the first snack right away.

Furthermore, the inability to delay gratification seemed to occur in a spontaneous and emotional manner, without

much thought. The children who could not resist simply grabbed the cookie because it looked so yummy, without

being able to stop themselves (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999; Strack & Deutsch, 2007).

The ability to regulate our emotions has important consequences later in life. When Mischel followed up on the

children in his original study, he found that those who had been able to self-regulate grew up to have some highly

positive characteristics: They got better university admission test scores, were rated by their friends as more socially

adept, and were found to cope with frustration and stress better than those children who could not resist the tempting

cookie at a young age. Thus effective self-regulation can be recognized as an important key to success in life (Ayduk

et al., 2000; Eigsti et al., 2006; Mischel & Ayduk, 2004).

Emotion regulation is influenced by body chemicals, particularly the neurotransmitter serotonin. Preferences for

small, immediate rewards over large but later rewards have been linked to low levels of serotonin in animals (Bizot,

Le Bihan, Peuch, Hamon, & Thiebot, 1999; Liu, Wilkinson, & Robbins, 2004), and low levels of serotonin are tied

to violence and impulsiveness in human suicides (Asberg, Traskman, & Thoren, 1976).

Research Focus: Emotion Regulation Takes Effort

Emotion regulation is particularly difficult when we are tired, depressed, or anxious, and it is under these

conditions that we more easily let our emotions get the best of us (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000). If you are

tired and worried about an upcoming exam, you may find yourself getting angry and taking it out on your

roommate, even though she really hasn’t done anything to deserve it and you don’t really want to be angry

at her. It is no secret that we are more likely fail at our diets when we are under a lot of stress, or at night

when we are tired.

Muraven, Tice, and Baumeister (1998) conducted a study to demonstrate that emotion regulation — that

is, either increasing or decreasing our emotional responses — takes work. They speculated that self-control

was like a muscle; it just gets tired when it is used too much. In their experiment they asked participants

to watch a short movie about environmental disasters involving radioactive waste and their negative effects

on wildlife. The scenes included sick and dying animals and were very upsetting. According to random

assignment to condition, one group (the increase emotional response condition) was told to really get into

the movie and to express their emotions, one group was to hold back and decrease their emotional responses

(the decrease emotional response condition), and the third (control) group received no emotional regulation

instructions.

Both before and after the movie, the experimenter asked the participants to engage in a measure of physical

strength by squeezing as hard as they could on a handgrip exerciser, a device used for strengthening hand

muscles. The experimenter put a piece of paper in the grip and timed how long the participants could hold

the grip together before the paper fell out. Figure 11.9, “Research Results,” shows the results of this study.

11.2 STRESS: THE UNSEEN KILLER • 454

It seems that emotion regulation does indeed take effort, because the participants who had been asked to

control their emotions showed significantly less ability to squeeze the handgrip after the movie than they had

showed before it, whereas the control group showed virtually no decrease. The emotion regulation during the

movie seems to have consumed resources, leaving the participants with less capacity to perform the handgrip

task.

Figure 11.9 Research Results. Participants who were instructed to regulate their emotions, either

by increasing or decreasing their emotional responses to a move, had less energy left over to

squeeze a handgrip in comparison to those who did not regulate their emotions. Adapted from

Muraven, Tice, & Baumeister, 1998.

In other studies, people who had to resist the temptation to eat chocolates and cookies, who made important

decisions, or who were forced to conform to others all performed more poorly on subsequent tasks that took

energy, including giving up on tasks earlier and failing to resist temptation (Vohs & Heatherton, 2000).

Can we improve our emotion regulation? It turns out that training in self-regulation —just like physical training—

can help. Students who practised doing difficult tasks, such as exercising, avoiding swearing, or maintaining good

posture, were later found to perform better in laboratory tests of emotion regulation such as maintaining a diet or

completing a puzzle (Baumeister, Gailliot, DeWall, & Oaten, 2006; Baumeister, Schmeichel, & Vohs, 2007; Oaten

& Cheng, 2006).

Key Takeaways

• Stress refers to the physiological responses that occur when an organism fails to respond

appropriately to emotional or physical threats.

• The general adaptation syndrome refers to the three distinct phases of physiological change that

occur in response to long-term stress: alarm, resistance, and exhaustion.

• Stress is normally adaptive because it helps us respond to potentially dangerous events by

activating the sympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system. But the experience of

prolonged stress has a direct negative influence on our physical health.

• Chronic stress is a major contributor to heart disease. It also decreases our ability to fight off colds

and infections.

455 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

• Stressors can occur as a result of both major and minor everyday events.

• Men tend to respond to stress with the fight-or-flight response, whereas women are more likely to

take a tend-and-befriend response.

Exercises and Critical Thinking

1. Consider a time when you experienced stress and how you responded to it. Do you now have a

better understanding of the dangers of stress? How will you change your coping mechanisms

based on what you have learned?

2. Are you good at emotion regulation? Can you think of a time when your emotions got the better

of you? How might you make better use of your emotions?

References

Asberg, M., Traskman, L., & Thoren, P. (1976). 5-HIAA in the cerebrospinal fluid: A biochemical suicide

predictor? Archives of General Psychiatry, 33(10), 1193–1197.

Ayduk, O., Mendoza-Denton, R., Mischel, W., Downey, G., Peake, P. K., & Rodriguez, M. (2000). Regulating the

interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 79(5), 776–792.

Baumeister, R. F., Gailliot, M., DeWall, C. N., & Oaten, M. (2006). Self-regulation and personality: How

interventions increase regulatory success, and how depletion moderates the effects of traits on behavior. Journal of

Personality, 74(6), 1773–1801.

Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B., & Vohs, K. D. (2007). Self-regulation and the executive function: The self

as controlling agent. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic

principles (Vol. 2). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Bizot, J.-C., Le Bihan, C., Peuch, A. J., Hamon, M., & Thiebot, M.-H. (1999). Serotonin and tolerance to delay of

reward in rats. Psychopharmacology, 146(4), 400–412.

Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Malarkey, W. B., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Sheridan, J. F., Poehlmann, K.

M.,…Glaser, R. (1998). Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and immune responses to psychological stress: The reactivity

hypothesis. In Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Neuroimmunomodulation: Molecular aspects,

integrative systems, and clinical advances (Vol. 840, pp. 664–673). New York, NY: New York Academy of

Sciences.

Canadian Mental Health Association. (2014). Post traumatic stress disorder. Retrieved 2014 from

http://www.cmha.bc.ca/get-informed/mental-health-information/ptsd

11.2 STRESS: THE UNSEEN KILLER • 456

Cohen, S., & Herbert, T. B. (1996). Health psychology: Psychological factors and physical disease from the

perspective of human psychoneuroimmunology. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 113–142.

Dekker, M., Koper, J., van Aken, M., Pols, H., Hofman, A., de Jong, F.,…Tiemeier, H. (2008). Salivary cortisol is

related to atherosclerosis of carotid arteries. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 93(10), 3741.

Eigsti, I.-M., Zayas, V., Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., Ayduk, O., Dadlani, M. B.,…Casey, B. J. (2006). Predicting

cognitive control from preschool to late adolescence and young adulthood. Psychological Science, 17(6), 478–484.

Epel, E., Lin, J., Wilhelm, F., Wolkowitz, O., Cawthon, R., Adler, N.,…Blackburn, E. H. (2006). Cell aging in

relation to stress arousal and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 31(3), 277–287.

Faulkner, S., & Smith, A. (2009). A prospective diary study of the role of psychological stress and negative mood

in the recurrence of herpes simplex virus (HSV1). Stress and Health: Journal of the International Society for the

Investigation of Stress, 25(2), 179–187.

Fiksenbaum, L. M., Greenglass, E. R., & Eaton, J. (2006). Perceived social support, hassles, and coping among the

elderly. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 25(1), 17–30.

Friedman, M., & Rosenman, R. H. (1974). Type A behavior and your heart. New York, NY: Knopf.

Glaser, R. (1985). Stress-related impairments in cellular immunity. Psychiatry Research, 16(3), 233–239.

Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive

emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(1), 95–103.

Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11,

213–218.

Hutchinson, J. G., & Williams, P. G. (2007). Neuroticism, daily hassles, and depressive symptoms: An examination

of moderating and mediating effects. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(7), 1367–1378.

Kelsey, R. M., Blascovich, J., Tomaka, J., Leitten, C. L., Schneider, T. R., & Wiens, S. (1999). Cardiovascular

reactivity and adaptation to recurrent psychological stress: Effects of prior task exposure. Psychophysiology, 36(6),

818–831.

Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., McGuire, L., Robles, T. F., & Glaser, R. (2002). Psychoneuroimmunology: Psychological

influences on immune function and health. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 70(3), 537–547.

Krantz, D. S., & McCeney, M. K. (2002). Effects of psychological and social factors on organic disease: A critical

assessment of research on coronary heart disease. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 341–369.

Liu, Y. P., Wilkinson, L. S., & Robbins, T. W. (2004). Effects of acute and chronic buspirone on impulsive choice

and efflux of 5-HT and dopamine in hippocampus, nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology,

173(1–2), 175–185.

McIntyre, K., Korn, J., & Matsuo, H. (2008). Sweating the small stuff: How different types of hassles result in the

experience of stress. Stress & Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress, 24(5),

383–392.

Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis of delay of gratification: Dynamics of

willpower. Psychological Review, 106(1), 3–19.

457 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Miller, G., Chen, E., & Cole, S. W. (2009). Health psychology: Developing biologically plausible models linking

the social world and physical health. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 501–524.

Mischel, W., & Ayduk, O. (Eds.). (2004). Willpower in a cognitive-affective processing system: The dynamics of

delay of gratification. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control

resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 247–259.

Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion

patterns. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 74(3), 774–789.

Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006). Longitudinal gains in self-regulation from regular physical exercise. British Journal

of Health Psychology, 11(4), 717–733.

Pennebaker, J. W., & Beall, S. K. (1986). Confronting a traumatic event: Toward an understanding of inhibition and

disease. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(3), 274–281.

Pennebaker, J. W., & Stone, L. D. (Eds.). (2004). Translating traumatic experiences into language: Implications

for child abuse and long-term health. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Pennebaker, J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerating the coping process. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 58(3), 528–537.

Petrie, K. J., Fontanilla, I., Thomas, M. G., Booth, R. J., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Effect of written emotional

expression on immune function in patients with human immunodeficiency virus infection: A randomized

trial. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(2), 272–275.

Pulcino, T., Galea, S., Ahern, J., Resnick, H., Foley, M., & Vlahov, D. (2003). Posttraumatic stress in women after

the September 11 terrorist attacks in New York City. Journal of Women’s Health, 12(8), 809–820.

Rahe, R. H., Veach, T. L.,Tolles, Robbyn L., and Murakami, K. (2000). The stress and coping inventory: an

educational and research instrument. Stress Medicine, 16(4), p.199–208.

Rahe, R. H., Mahan, J., Arthur, R. J., & Gunderson, E. K. E. (1970). The epidemiology of illness in naval

environments: I. Illness types, distribution, severities and relationships to life change. Military Medicine, 135,

443–452.

Rodrigues, S. M., LeDoux, J. E., & Sapolsky, R. M. (2009). The influence of stress hormones on fear

circuitry. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 289–313.

Selye, H. (1936). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents [PDF]. Nature, 138, 32. Retrieved

from http://neuro.psychiatryonline.org/cgi/reprint/10/2/230a.pdf

Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Selye, H. (1974). Forty years of stress research: Principal remaining problems and misconceptions. Canadian

Medical Association Journal, 115(1), 53–56.

Selye, H. (1982). The nature of stress. Retrieved from http://www.icnr.com/articles/thenatureofstress.html

11.2 STRESS: THE UNSEEN KILLER • 458

Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2007). The role of impulse in social behavior. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins

(Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (Vol. 2). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Suls, J., & Bunde, J. (2005). Anger, anxiety, and depression as risk factors for cardiovascular disease: The problems

and implications of overlapping affective dispositions. Psychological Bulletin, 131(2), 260–300.

Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000).

Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107(3),

411–429.

Uchino, B. N., Smith, T. W., Holt-Lunstad, J., Campo, R., & Reblin, M. (2007). Stress and illness. In J. T. Cacioppo,

L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 608–632). New York, NY:

Cambridge University Press.

Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological

Science, 11(3), 249–254.

Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of

negative affectivity. Psychological Review, 96(2), 234–254.

Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought

suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 5–13.

Wells, W. (2006). How chronic stress exacerbates cancer. Journal of Cell Biology, 174(4), 476.

Williams, R. B. (2001). Hostility: Effects on health and the potential for successful behavioral approaches to

prevention and treatment. In A. Baum, T. A. Revenson, & J. E. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of health psychology.

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Long Description

Figure 11.7 long description: Stages of Stress as identified by Hans Selye. Stage 1: General alarm reaction. The

first reaction to stress. The body releases stress hormones, including cortisol. Stage 2: Resistance. After a period of

chronic stress the body adapts to the ongoing threat and tries to return to normal functions. Glucose levels increase to

sustain energy, and blood pressure increases. Stage 3: Exhaustion. In this stage, the body has run out of its reserves

of energy and immunity. Blood sugar levels decrease, leading to decreased stress tolerance, progressive mental and

physical exhaustion, illness, and collapse. The body’s organs begin to fail, and eventually illness or death occurs.

[Return to Figure 11.7]

459 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

11.3 Positive Emotions: The Power of Happiness

Learning Objectives

1. Understand the important role of positive emotions and happiness in responding to stress.

2. Understand the factors that increase, and do not increase, happiness.

Although stress is an emotional response that can kill us, our emotions can also help us cope with and protect

ourselves from it. The stress of the Monday through Friday grind can be offset by the fun that we can have on the

weekend, and the concerns that we have about our upcoming chemistry exam can be offset by a positive attitude

toward school, life, and other people. Put simply, the best antidote for stress is a happy one: think positively, have

fun, and enjoy the company of others.

You have probably heard about the power of positive thinking—the idea that thinking positively helps people meet

their goals and keeps them healthy, happy, and able to effectively cope with the negative events that occur to them.

It turns out that positive thinking really works. People who think positively about their future, who believe that they

can control their outcomes, and who are willing to open up and share with others are healthier people (Seligman, &

Csikszentmihalyi, 2000).

The power of positive thinking comes in different forms, but they are all helpful. Some researchers have focused on

optimism, a general tendency to expect positive outcomes, finding that optimists are happier and have less stress

(Carver & Scheier, 2009). Others have focused on self-efficacy, the belief in our ability to carry out actions that

produce desired outcomes. People with high self-efficacy respond to environmental and other threats in an active,

constructive way — by getting information, talking to friends, and attempting to face and reduce the difficulties

they are experiencing. These people too are better able to ward off their stresses in comparison to people with less

self-efficacy (Thompson, 2009).

Self-efficacy helps in part because it leads us to perceive that we can control the potential stressors that may affect

us. Workers who have control over their work environment (e.g., by being able to move furniture and control

distractions) experience less stress, as do patients in nursing homes who are able to choose their everyday activities

(Rodin, 1986). Glass, Reim, and Singer (1971) found that participants who believed that they could stop a loud

noise experienced less stress than those who did not think that they could, even though the people who had the

option never actually used it. The ability to control our outcomes may help explain why animals and people who

have higher status live longer (Sapolsky, 2005).

Suzanne Kobasa and her colleagues (Kobasa, Maddi, & Kahn, 1982) have argued that the tendency to be less

affected by life’s stressors can be characterized as an individual difference measure that has a relationship to both

optimism and self-efficacy known as hardiness. Hardy individuals are those who are more positive overall about

potentially stressful life events, who take more direct action to understand the causes of negative events, and who

attempt to learn from them what may be of value for the future. Hardy individuals use effective coping strategies,

and they take better care of themselves.

460

Taken together, these various coping skills, including optimism, self-efficacy, and hardiness, have been shown to

have a wide variety of positive effects on our health. Optimists make faster recoveries from illnesses and surgeries

(Carver et al., 2005). People with high self-efficacy have been found to be better able to quit smoking and lose

weight and are more likely to exercise regularly (Cohen & Pressman, 2006). And hardy individuals seem to cope

better with stress and other negative life events (Dolbier, Smith, & Steinhardt, 2007). The positive effects of positive

thinking are particularly important when stress is high. Baker (2007) found that in periods of low stress, positive

thinking made little difference in responses to stress, but that during stressful periods optimists were less likely to

smoke on a day-to-day basis and to respond to stress in more productive ways, such as by exercising.

It is possible to learn to think more positively, and doing so can be beneficial. Antoni and colleagues (2001) found

that pessimistic cancer patients who were given training in optimism reported more optimistic outlooks after

the training and were less fatigued after their treatments. And Maddi, Kahn, and Maddi (1998) found that a

hardiness training program that included focusing on ways to effectively cope with stress was effective in increasing

satisfaction and decreasing self-reported stress.

The benefits of taking positive approaches to stress can last a lifetime. Christopher Peterson and his colleagues

(Peterson, Seligman, Yurko, Martin, & Friedman, 1998) found that the level of optimism reported by people who

had first been interviewed when they were in university during the years between 1936 and 1940 predicted their

health over the next 50 years. Students who had a more positive outlook on life in university were less likely to have

died up to 50 years later of all causes, and they were particularly likely to have experienced fewer accidental and

violent deaths, in comparison to students who were less optimistic. Similar findings were found for older adults.

After controlling for loneliness, marital status, economic status, and other correlates of health, Levy and Myers

found that older adults with positive attitudes and higher self-efficacy had better health and lived on average almost

eight years longer than their more negative peers (Levy & Myers, 2005; Levy, Slade, & Kasl, 2002). And Diener,

Nickerson, Lucas, and Sandvik (2002) found that people who had cheerier dispositions earlier in life had higher

income levels and less unemployment when they were assessed 19 years later.

Finding Happiness through Our Connections with Others

Happiness is determined in part by genetic factors, such that some people are naturally happier than others

(Braungart, Plomin, DeFries, & Fulker, 1992; Lykken, 2000), but also in part by the situations that we create for

ourselves. Psychologists have studied hundreds of variables that influence happiness, but there is one that is by far

the most important. People who report that they have positive social relationships with others — the perception of

social support — also report being happier than those who report having less social support (Diener, Suh, Lucas,

& Smith, 1999; Diener, Tamir, & Scollon, 2006). Married people report being happier than unmarried people (Pew,

2006), and people who are connected with and accepted by others suffer less depression, higher self-esteem, and

less social anxiety and jealousy than those who feel more isolated and rejected (Leary, 1990).

Social support also helps us better cope with stressors. Koopman, Hermanson, Diamond, Angell, and Spiegel

(1998) found that women who reported higher social support experienced less depression when adjusting to a

diagnosis of cancer, and Ashton and colleagues (2005) found a similar buffering effect of social support for AIDS

patients. People with social support are less depressed overall, recover faster from negative events, and are less

likely to commit suicide (Au, Lau, & Lee, 2009; Bertera, 2007; Compton, Thompson, & Kaslow, 2005; Sk.rs.ter,

Langius, .gren, H.agstr.m, & Dencker, 2005).

Social support buffers us against stress in several ways. For one, having people we can trust and rely on helps us

directly by allowing us to share favours when we need them. These are the direct effects of social support. But

having people around us also makes us feel good about ourselves. These are the appreciation effects of social

support. Gen..z and .zlale (2004) found that students with more friends felt less stress and reported that their

461 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

friends helped them, but they also reported that having friends made them feel better about themselves. Again, you

can see that the tend-and-befriend response, so often used by women, is an important and effective way to reduce

stress.

What Makes Us Happy?

One difficulty that people face when trying to improve their happiness is that they may not always know what will

make them happy. As one example, many of us think that if we just had more money we would be happier. While it

is true that we do need money to afford food and adequate shelter for ourselves and our families, after this minimum

level of wealth is reached, more money does not generally buy more happiness (Easterlin, 2005). For instance, as

you can see in Figure 11.10, “Income and Happiness,” even though income and material success has improved

dramatically in many countries over the past decades, happiness has not. Despite tremendous economic growth in

France, Japan, and Canada between 1946 and 1990, there was no increase in reports of well-being by the citizens

of these countries. People today have about three times the buying power they had in the 1950s, and yet overall

happiness has not increased. The problem seems to be that we never seem to have enough money to make us really

happy. Csikszentmihalyi (1999) reported that people who earned $30,000 per year felt that they would be happier if

they made $50,000 per year, but that people who earned $100,000 per year said that they would need $250,000 per

year to make them happy.

Figure 11.10 Income and Happiness. Although personal income keeps rising, happiness does not.

These findings might lead us to conclude that we don’t always know what does or what might make us happy, and

this seems to be at least partially true. For instance, Jean Twenge and her colleagues (Twenge, Campbell & Foster,

2003) have found in several studies that although people with children frequently claim that having children makes

them happy, couples who do not have children actually report being happier than those who do.

Psychologists have found that people’s ability to predict their future emotional states is not very accurate (Wilson &

Gilbert, 2005). For one, people overestimate their emotional reactions to events. Although people think that positive

11.3 POSITIVE EMOTIONS: THE POWER OF HAPPINESS • 462

and negative events that might occur to them will make a huge difference in their lives, and although these changes

do make at least some difference in life satisfaction, they tend to be less influential than we think they are going

to be. Positive events tend to make us feel good, but their effects wear off pretty quickly, and the same is true for

negative events. For instance, Brickman, Coates, and Janoff-Bulman (1978) interviewed people who had won more

than $50,000 in a lottery and found that they were not happier than they had been in the past, and were also not

happier than a control group of similar people who had not won the lottery. On the other hand, the researchers found

that individuals who were paralyzed as a result of accidents were not as unhappy as might be expected.

How can this possibly be? There are several reasons. For one, people are resilient; they bring their coping skills

to play when negative events occur, and this makes them feel better. Secondly, most people do not continually

experience very positive, or very negative, affect over a long period of time, but rather adapt to their current

circumstances. Just as we enjoy the second chocolate bar we eat less than we enjoy the first, as we experience

more and more positive outcomes in our daily lives we habituate to them and our life satisfaction returns to a more

moderate level (Small, Zatorre, Dagher, Evans, & Jones-Gotman, 2001).

Another reason that we may mispredict our happiness is that our social comparisons change when our own status

changes as a result of new events. People who are wealthy compare themselves to other wealthy people, people

who are poor tend to compare with other poor people, and people who are ill tend to compare with other ill people.

When our comparisons change, our happiness levels are correspondingly influenced. And when people are asked to

predict their future emotions, they may focus only on the positive or negative event they are asked about, and forget

about all the other things that won’t change. Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, Gilbert, and Axsom (2000) found that when

people were asked to focus on all the more regular things that they will still be doing in the future (working, going

to church, socializing with family and friends, and so forth), their predictions about how something really good or

bad would influence them were less extreme.

If pleasure is fleeting, at least misery shares some of the same quality. We might think we cannot be happy if

something terrible, such as the loss of a partner or child, were to happen to us, but after a period of adjustment most

people find that happiness levels return to prior levels (Bonnano et al., 2002). Health concerns tend to put a damper

on our feeling of well-being, and those with a serious disability or illness show slightly lowered mood levels. But

even when health is compromised, levels of misery are lower than most people expect (Lucas, 2007; Riis et al.,

2005). For instance, although disabled individuals have more concern about health, safety, and acceptance in the

community, they still experience overall positive happiness levels (Marinić & Brkljačić, 2008). Taken together, it

has been estimated that our wealth, health, and life circumstances account for only 15% to 20% of life satisfaction

scores (Argyle, 1999). Clearly the main ingredient in happiness lies beyond, or perhaps beneath, external factors.

Key Takeaways

• Positive thinking can be beneficial to our health.

• Optimism, self-efficacy, and hardiness all relate to positive health outcomes.

• Happiness is determined in part by genetic factors, but also by the experience of social support.

• People may not always know what will make them happy.

• Material wealth plays only a small role in determining happiness.

463 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Exercises and Critical Thinking

1. Are you a happy person? Can you think of ways to increase your positive emotions?

2. Do you know what will make you happy? Do you believe that material wealth is not as

important as you might have thought it would be?

References

Antoni, M. H., Lehman, J. M., Klibourn, K. M., Boyers, A. E., Culver, J. L., Alferi, S. M.,…Kilbourn, K. (2001).

Cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention decreases the prevalence of depression and enhances benefit

finding among women under treatment for early-stage breast cancer. Health Psychology, 20(1), 20–32.

Argyle, M. (1999). Causes and correlates of happiness. In D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well

being: The foundations of hedonic psychology. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Ashton, E., Vosvick, M., Chesney, M., Gore-Felton, C., Koopman, C., O’Shea, K.,…Spiegel, D. (2005). Social

support and maladaptive coping as predictors of the change in physical health symptoms among persons living with

HIV/AIDS. AIDS Patient Care & STDs, 19(9), 587–598.

Au, A., Lau, S., & Lee, M. (2009). Suicide ideation and depression: The moderation effects of family cohesion and

social self-concept. Adolescence, 44(176), 851–868. Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

Baker, S. R. (2007). Dispositional optimism and health status, symptoms, and behaviors: Assessing ideothetic

relationships using a prospective daily diary approach. Psychology and Health, 22(4), 431–455.

Bertera, E. (2007). The role of positive and negative social exchanges between adolescents, their peers and family

as predictors of suicide ideation. Child & Adolescent Social Work Journal, 24(6), 523–538.

Bonanno, G. A., Wortman, C. B., Lehman, D. R., Tweed, R. G., Haring, M., Sonnega, J.,…Nesse, R. M. (2002).

Resilience to loss and chronic grief: A prospective study from preloss to 18-months postloss. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1150–1164.

Braungart, J. M., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1992). Genetic influence on tester-rated infant

temperament as assessed by Bayley’s Infant Behavior Record: Nonadoptive and adoptive siblings and

twins. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 40–47.

Brickman, P., Coates, D., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1978). Lottery winners and accident victims: Is happiness

relative? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(8), 917–927.

Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2009). Optimism. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of individual

differences in social behavior (pp. 330–342). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Carver, C. S., Smith, R. G., Antoni, M. H., Petronis, V. M., Weiss, S., & Derhagopian, R. P. (2005). Optimistic

personality and psychosocial well-being during treatment predict psychosocial well-being among long-term

survivors of breast cancer. Health Psychology, 24(5), 508–516.

11.3 POSITIVE EMOTIONS: THE POWER OF HAPPINESS • 464

Cohen, S., & Pressman, S. D. (2006). Positive affect and health. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3),

122–125.

Compton, M., Thompson, N., & Kaslow, N. (2005). Social environment factors associated with suicide attempt

among low-income African Americans: The protective role of family relationships and social support. Social

Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40(3), 175–185.

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). If we are so rich, why aren’t we happy? American Psychologist, 54(10), 821–827.

Diener, E., Nickerson, C., Lucas, R., & Sandvik, E. (2002). Dispositional affect and job outcomes. Social Indicators

Research, 59(3), 229.

Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three decades of

progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276–302.

Diener, E., Tamir, M., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). Happiness, life satisfaction, and fulfillment: The social psychology

of subjective well-being. In P. A. M. VanLange (Ed.), Bridging social psychology: Benefits of transdisciplinary

approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Dolbier, C. L., Smith, S. E., & Steinhardt, M. A. (2007). Relationships of protective factors to stress and symptoms

of illness. American Journal of Health Behavior, 31(4), 423–433.

Easterlin, R. (2005). Feeding the illusion of growth and happiness: A reply to Hagerty and Veenhoven. Social

Indicators Research, 74(3), 429–443. doi:10.1007/s11205-004-6170-z

Gen..z, T., & .zlale, Y. (2004). Direct and indirect effects of social support on psychological well-being. Social

Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, 32(5), 449–458.

Glass, D. C., Reim, B., & Singer, J. E. (1971). Behavioral consequences of adaptation to controllable and

uncontrollable noise. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7(2), 244–257.

Kobasa, S. C., Maddi, S. R., & Kahn, S. (1982). Hardiness and health: A prospective study. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 42(1), 168–177.

Koopman, C., Hermanson, K., Diamond, S., Angell, K., & Spiegel, D. (1998). Social support, life stress, pain and

emotional adjustment to advanced breast cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 7(2), 101–110.

Layard, R. (2005). Happiness: Lessons from a new science. New York, NY: Penguin.

Leary, M. R. (1990). Responses to social exclusion: Social anxiety, jealousy, loneliness, depression, and low selfesteem.

Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 9(2), 221–229.

Levy, B., & Myers, L. (2005). Relationship between respiratory mortality and self-perceptions of aging. Psychology

& Health, 20(5), 553–564. doi:10.1080/14768320500066381.

Levy, B., Slade, M., & Kasl, S. (2002). Longitudinal benefit of positive self-perceptions of aging on functional

health. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, 57B(5), P409. Retrieved from

Academic Search Premier Database.

Lucas, R. (2007). Long-term disability is associated with lasting changes in subjective well-being: Evidence from

two nationally representative longitudinal studies. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 92(4), 717–730.

Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

465 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Lykken, D. T. (2000). Happiness: The nature and nurture of joy and contentment. New York, NY: St. Martin’s

Press.

Maddi, S. R., Kahn, S., & Maddi, K. L. (1998). The effectiveness of hardiness training. Consulting Psychology

Journal: Practice and Research, 50(2), 78–86.

Marinić, M., & Brkljačić, T. (2008). Love over gold—The correlation of happiness level with some life satisfaction

factors between persons with and without physical disability. Journal of Developmental & Physical Disabilities,

20(6), 527–540.

Peterson, C., Seligman, M. E. P., Yurko, K. H., Martin, L. R., & Friedman, H. S. (1998). Catastrophizing and

untimely death. Psychological Science, 9(2), 127–130.

Pew Research Center (2006, February 13). Are we happy yet? Retrieved from http://pewresearch.org/pubs/301/arewe-

happy-yet

Riis, J., Baron, J., Loewenstein, G., Jepson, C., Fagerlin, A., & Ubel, P. (2005). Ignorance of hedonic adaptation

to hemodialysis: A study using ecological momentary assessment. Journal of Experimental Psychology/General,

134(1), 3–9.

Rodin, J. (1986). Aging and health: Effects of the sense of control. Science, 233(4770), 1271–1276.

Sapolsky, R. M. (2005). The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science, 308(5722), 648–652.

Seligman, M. E. P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist,

55(1), 5–14.

Sk.rs.ter, I., Langius, A., .gren, H., H.agstr.m, L., & Dencker, K. (2005). Sense of coherence and social support

in relation to recovery in first-episode patients with major depression: A one-year prospective study. International

Journal of Mental Health Nursing, 14(4), 258–264.

Small, D. M., Zatorre, R. J., Dagher, A., Evans, A. C., & Jones-Gotman, M. (2001). Changes in brain activity related

to eating chocolate: From pleasure to aversion. Brain, 124(9), 1720–1733.

Thompson, S. C. (2009). The role of personal control in adaptive functioning. In S. J. Lopez & C. R. Snyder

(Eds.), Oxford handbook of positive psychology (2nd ed., pp. 271–278). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., & Foster, C. A. (2003). Parenthood and marital satisfaction: A meta-analytic

review. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65(3), 574–583.

Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want. Current Directions in

Psychological Science, 14(3), 131–134.

Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000). Focalism: A source of durability

bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 821–836.

Image Attributions

Figure 11.10: Layard, 2005.

11.3 POSITIVE EMOTIONS: THE POWER OF HAPPINESS • 466

11.4 Two Fundamental Human Motivations: Eating and Mating

Learning Objectives

1. Understand the biological and social responses that underlie eating behaviour.

2. Understand the psychological and physiological responses that underlie sexual behaviour.

Eating: Healthy Choices Make Healthy Lives

Along with the need to drink fresh water, which humans can normally attain in all except the most extreme

situations, the need for food is the most fundamental and important human need. More than one in 10

Canadian households contain people who live without enough nourishing food, and this lack of proper nourishment

has profound effects on their abilities to lead productive lives (Yarema, 2013). When people are extremely hungry,

their motivation to attain food completely changes their behaviour. Hungry people become listless and apathetic

to save energy and then become completely obsessed with food. Ancel Keys and his colleagues (Keys, Brožek,

Henschel, Mickelsen, & Taylor, 1950) found that volunteers who were placed on severely reduced-calorie diets lost

all interest in sex and social activities, becoming preoccupied with food.

Like most interesting psychological phenomena, the simple behaviour of eating has both biological and social

determinants (Figure 11.11, “Biological, Psychological, and Social-Cultural Contributors to Eating”). Biologically,

hunger is controlled by the interactions among complex pathways in the nervous system and a variety of hormonal

and chemical systems in the brain and body. The stomach is of course important. We feel more hungry when our

stomach is empty than when it is full. But we can also feel hunger even without input from the stomach. Two

areas of the hypothalamus are known to be particularly important in eating. The lateral part of the hypothalamus

responds primarily to cues to start eating, whereas the ventromedial part of the hypothalamus primarily responds

to cues to stop eating. If the lateral part of the hypothalamus is damaged, the animal will not eat even if food is

present, whereas if the ventromedial part of the hypothalamus is damaged, the animal will eat until it is obese (Wolf

& Miller, 1964).

Hunger is also determined by hormone levels (Figure 11.12, “Eating Is Influenced by the Appetite Hormones”).

Glucose is the main sugar that the body uses for energy, and the brain monitors blood glucose levels to determine

hunger. Glucose levels in the bloodstream are regulated by insulin, a hormone secreted by the pancreas gland.

When insulin is low, glucose is not taken up by body cells, and the body begins to use fat as an energy source. Eating

and appetite are also influenced by other hormones, including orexin, ghrelin, and leptin (Brennan & Mantzoros,

2006; Nakazato et al., 2001).

Normally the interaction of the various systems that determine hunger creates a balance or homeostasis in which

we eat when we are hungry and stop eating when we feel full. But homeostasis varies among people; some people

467

Figure 11.11 Biological, Psychological, and Social-Cultural Contributors to Eating. [Long

Description]

simply weigh more than others, and there is little they can do to change their fundamental weight. Weight is

determined in large part by the basal metabolic rate, the amount of energy expended while at rest. Each person’s

basal metabolic rate is different, due to his or her unique physical makeup and physical behaviour. A naturally

occurring low metabolic rate, which is determined entirely by genetics, makes weight management a very difficult

undertaking for many people.

How we eat is also influenced by our environment. When researchers rigged clocks to move faster, people got

hungrier and ate more, as if they thought they must be hungry again because so much time had passed since they

last ate (Schachter, 1968). And if we forget that we have already eaten, we are likely to eat again even if we are not

actually hungry (Rozin, Dow, Moscovitch, & Rajaram, 1998).

Cultural norms about appropriate weights also influence eating behaviours. Current norms for women in Western

societies are based on a very thin body ideal, emphasized by television and movie actresses, models, and even

children’s dolls, such as the ever-popular Barbie. These norms for excessive thinness are very difficult for most

women to attain: Barbie’s measurements, if translated to human proportions, would be about 91 cm-46 cm-84 cm at

bust-waist-hips, measurements that are attained by less than one in 100,000 women (Norton, Olds, Olive, & Dank,

1996). Many women idealize being thin and yet are unable to reach the standard that they prefer.

Eating Disorders

In some cases, the desire to be thin can lead to eating disorders, which are estimated to affect about 1% of people,

90% of them women (Hoek & van Hoeken, 2003; Palmer, 2008; Patrick, 2002). Anorexia nervosa is an eating

disorder characterized by extremely low body weight, distorted body image, and an obsessive fear of gaining

weight. Anorexia begins with a severe weight loss diet and develops into a preoccupation with food and dieting.

11.4 TWO FUNDAMENTAL HUMAN MOTIVATIONS: EATING AND MATING • 468

Figure 11.12 Eating Is Influenced by the Appetite Hormones. Insulin, secreted by the pancreas,

controls blood glucose; leptin, secreted by fat cells, monitors energy levels; orexin, secreted by

the hypothalamus, triggers hunger; ghrelin, secreted by an empty stomach, increases food intake.

Bulimia nervosa is an eating disorder characterized by binge eating followed by purging. Bulimia nervosa begins

after the dieter has broken a diet and gorged. Bulimia involves repeated episodes of overeating, followed by

vomiting, laxative use, fasting, or excessive exercise. It is most common in women in their late teens or early 20s,

and it is often accompanied by depression and anxiety, particularly around the time of the binging. The cycle in

which the person eats to feel better, but then after eating becomes concerned about weight gain and purges, repeats

itself over and over again, often with major psychological and physical results.

Eating disorders are in part heritable (Klump, Burt, McGue, & Iacono, 2007), and it is not impossible that at

least some have been selected through their evolutionary significance in coping with food shortages (Guisinger,

2008). Eating disorders are also related to psychological causes, including low self-esteem, perfectionism, and the

perception that one’s body weight is too high (Vohs et al., 2001), as well as to cultural norms about body weight and

eating (Crandall, 1988). Because eating disorders can create profound negative health outcomes, including death,

people who suffer from them should seek treatment. This treatment is often quite effective.

Obesity

Although some people eat too little, eating too much is also a major problem. Obesity is a medical condition

in which so much excess body fat has accumulated in the body that it begins to have an adverse impact on

health. In addition to causing people to be stereotyped and treated less positively by others (Crandall, Merman,

& Hebl, 2009), uncontrolled obesity leads to health problems including cardiovascular disease, diabetes, sleep

469 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

apnea, arthritis, Alzheimer’s disease, and some types of cancer (Gustafson, Rothenberg, Blennow, Steen, & Skoog,

2003). Obesity also reduces life expectancy (Haslam & James, 2005).

Obesity is determined by calculating the body mass index (BMI), a measurement that compares one’s weight and

height. People are defined as overweight when their BMI is greater than 25 kg/m2 and as obese when it is greater

than 30 kg/m2. If you know your height and weight, you can calculate your BMI (https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health/

educational/lose_wt/BMI/bmicalc.htm)

Obesity is a leading cause of death worldwide. Its prevalence is rapidly increasing, and it is one of the most

serious public health problems of the 21st century. Although obesity is caused in part by genetics, it is increased by

overeating and a lack of physical activity (James, 2008; Nestle & Jacobson, 2000).

There are really only two approaches to controlling weight: eat less and exercise more. Dieting is difficult for

anyone, but it is particularly difficult for people with slow basal metabolic rates who must cope with severe hunger

to lose weight. Although most weight loss can be maintained for about a year, very few people are able to maintain

substantial weight loss through dieting alone for more than three years (Miller, 1999). Substantial weight loss

of more than 50 pounds is typically seen only when weight loss surgery has been performed (Douketis, Macie,

Thabane, & Williamson, 2005). Weight loss surgery reduces stomach volume or bowel length, leading to earlier

satiation and reduced ability to absorb nutrients from food.

Although dieting alone does not produce a great deal of weight loss over time, its effects are substantially improved

when it is accompanied by more physical activity. People who exercise regularly, and particularly those who

combine exercise with dieting, are less likely to be obese (Borer, 2008). Exercise not only improves our waistline

but also makes us healthier overall. Exercise increases cardiovascular capacity, lowers blood pressure, and helps

improve diabetes, joint flexibility, and muscle strength (American Heart Association, 1998). Exercise also slows the

cognitive impairments that are associated with aging (Kramer, Erickson, & Colcombe, 2006).

Because the costs of exercise are immediate but the benefits are long-term, it may be difficult for people who do not

exercise to get started. It is important to make a regular schedule, to work exercise into one’s daily activities, and to

view exercise not as a cost but as an opportunity to improve oneself (Schomer & Drake, 2001). Exercising is more

fun when it is done in groups, so team exercise is recommended (Kirchhoff, Elliott, Schlichting, & Chin, 2008).

A recent report found that only about one-half of North Americans perform the 30 minutes of exercise five times a

week that the World Health Organization (2010) suggests as the minimum healthy amount. As for the other half of

North Americans, they most likely are listening to the guidelines, but they are unable to stick to the regimen. Almost

half of the people who start an exercise regimen give it up by the six-month mark (American Heart Association,

1998; Colley, Garriguet, Janssen, Craig, Clarke, & Tremblay, 2011).

Statistics Canada (2010) also reports just 7% of young people between the ages of five and 17 meet the daily

recommended amount of physical activity. New international and proposed Canadian guidelines recommend adults

accumulate at least 150 minutes of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity a week to obtain substantial health

benefits. The guidelines also suggest five- to 17-year-olds should accumulate at least 60 minutes of moderate-tovigorous

physical activity daily. Statistics Canada says the guidelines for adults were achieved by 17% of men and

14% of women, while the youth guidelines were met by just 9% of boys and 4% of girls. This is a problem, given

that exercise has long-term benefits only if it is continued.

Sex: The Most Important Human Behaviour

Perhaps the most important aspect of human experience is the process of reproduction. Without it, none of us would

11.4 TWO FUNDAMENTAL HUMAN MOTIVATIONS: EATING AND MATING • 470

be here. Successful reproduction in humans involves the coordination of a wide variety of behaviours, including

courtship, sex, household arrangements, parenting, and child care.

The Experience of Sex

The sexual drive, with its reward of intense pleasure in orgasm, is highly motivating. The biology of the sexual

response was studied in detail by Masters and Johnson (1966), who monitored or filmed more than 700 men and

women while they masturbated or had intercourse. Masters and Johnson found that the sexual response cycle—the

biological sexual response in humans—was very similar in men and women, and consisted of four stages:

1. Excitement. The genital areas become engorged with blood. Women’s breasts and nipples may

enlarge and the vagina expands and secretes lubricant.

2. Plateau. Breathing, pulse, and blood pressure increase as orgasm feels imminent. The penis becomes

fully enlarged. Vaginal secretions continue and the clitoris may retract.

3. Orgasm. Muscular contractions occur throughout the body, but particularly in the genitals. The

spasmodic ejaculations of sperm are similar to the spasmodic contractions of vaginal walls, and the

experience of orgasm is similar for men and women. The woman’s orgasm helps position the uterus to

draw sperm inward (Thornhill & Gangestad, 1995).

4. Resolution. After orgasm the body gradually returns to its prearoused state. After one orgasm, men

typically experience a refractory period, in which they are incapable of reaching another orgasm for

several minutes, hours, or even longer. Women may achieve several orgasms before entering the

resolution stage.

The sexual response cycle and sexual desire are regulated by the sex hormones estrogen in women and testosterone

in both women and in men. Although the hormones are secreted by the ovaries and testes, it is the hypothalamus

and the pituitary glands that control the process. Estrogen levels in women vary across the menstrual cycle, peaking

during ovulation (Pillsworth, Haselton, & Buss, 2004). Women are more interested in having sex during ovulation

but can experience high levels of sexual arousal throughout the menstrual cycle.

In men, testosterone is essential to maintain sexual desire and to sustain an erection, and testosterone injections

can increase sexual interest and performance (Aversa et al., 2000; Jockenh.vel et al., 2009). Testosterone is also

important in the female sex cycle. Women who are experiencing menopause may develop a loss of interest in sex,

but this interest may be rekindled through estrogen and testosterone replacement treatments (Meston & Frohlich,

2000).

Although their biological determinants and experiences of sex are similar, men and women differ substantially in

their overall interest in sex, the frequency of their sexual activities, and the mates they are most interested in. Men

show a more consistent interest in sex, whereas the sexual desires of women are more likely to vary over time

(Baumeister, 2000). Men fantasize about sex more often than women, and their fantasies are more physical and less

intimate (Leitenberg & Henning, 1995). Men are also more willing to have casual sex than are women, and their

standards for sex partners is lower (Petersen & Hyde, 2010; Saad, Eba, & Sejean, 2009).

Gender differences in sexual interest probably occur in part as a result of the evolutionary predispositions of men

and women, and this interpretation is bolstered by the finding that gender differences in sexual interest are observed

cross-culturally (Buss, 1989). Evolutionarily, women should be more selective than men in their choices of sex

partners because they must invest more time in bearing and nurturing their children than men do (most men do help

out, of course, but women simply do more [Buss & Kenrick, 1998]). Because they do not need to invest a lot of time

471 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

in child rearing, men may be evolutionarily predisposed to be more willing and desiring of having sex with many

different partners and may be less selective in their choice of mates. Women, on the other hand, because they must

invest substantial effort in raising each child, should be more selective.

The Many Varieties of Sexual Behaviour

Sex researchers have found that sexual behaviour varies widely, not only between men and women but within each

sex (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948/1998; Kinsey, 1953/1998). About a quarter of women report having a low

sexual desire, and about 1% of people report feeling no sexual attraction whatsoever (Bogaert, 2004; Feldhaus-

Dahir, 2009; West et al., 2008). There are also people who experience hyperactive sexual drives. For about 3% to

6% of the population (mainly men), the sex drive is so strong that it dominates life experience and may lead to

hyperactive sexual desire disorder (Kingston & Firestone, 2008).

There is also variety in sexual orientation, which is the direction of our sexual desire toward people of the opposite

sex, people of the same sex, or people of both sexes. The vast majority of human beings have a heterosexual

orientation — their sexual desire is focused toward members of the opposite sex. A smaller minority is primarily

homosexual (i.e., they have sexual desire for members of their own sex). Between 3% and 4% of men are gay, and

between 1% and 2% of women are lesbian. Another 1% of the population reports being bisexual (having desires for

both sexes). The love and sexual lives of homosexuals are little different from those of heterosexuals, except where

their behaviours are constrained by cultural norms and local laws. As with heterosexuals, some gays and lesbians are

celibate, some are promiscuous, but most are in committed, long-term relationships (Laumann, Gagnon, Michael,

& Michaels, 1994).

Although homosexuality has been practiced as long as records of human behaviour have been kept, and occurs

in many animals at least as frequently as it does in humans, cultures nevertheless vary substantially in their

attitudes toward it. In Western societies such as the Canada, the United States, and Europe, attitudes are becoming

progressively more tolerant of homosexuality, but it remains unacceptable in many other parts of the world. The

Canadian Psychological Association issued a policy statement in February 1982 endorsing the principle that there

be no discrimination on the basis of sexual orientation for (a) recruitment; (b) hiring; (c) salary; (d) promotion

rate; (e) fringe benefits; or (f) assignment of duties. The American Psychiatric Association no longer considers

homosexuality to be a mental illness, although it did so until 1973. Because prejudice against gays and lesbians can

lead to experiences of ostracism, depression, and even suicide (Kulkin, Chauvin, & Percle, 2000), these improved

attitudes can benefit the everyday lives of gays, lesbians, and bisexuals.

Whether sexual orientation is driven more by nature or by nurture has received a great deal of research attention,

and research has found that sexual orientation is primarily biological (Mustanski, Chivers, & Bailey, 2002). Areas

of the hypothalamus are different in homosexual men, as well as in animals with homosexual tendencies, than they

are in heterosexual members of the species, and these differences are in directions such that gay men are more

similar to women than straight men are (Gladue, 1994; Lasco, Jordan, Edgar, Petito, & Byrne, 2002; Rahman &

Wilson, 2003). Twin studies also support the idea that there is a genetic component to sexual orientation. Among

male identical twins, 52% of those with a gay brother also reported homosexuality, whereas the rate in fraternal

twins was just 22% (Bailey et al., 1999; Pillard & Bailey, 1998). There is also evidence that sexual orientation is

influenced by exposure and responses to sex hormones (Hershberger & Segal, 2004; Williams & Pepitone, 2000).

Psychology in Everyday Life: Regulating Emotions to Improve Our Health

Although smoking cigarettes, drinking alcohol, using recreational drugs, engaging in unsafe sex, and eating

11.4 TWO FUNDAMENTAL HUMAN MOTIVATIONS: EATING AND MATING • 472

too much may produce enjoyable positive emotions in the short term, they are some of the leading causes

of negative health outcomes and even death in the long term (Mokdad, Marks, Stroup, & Gerberding,

2004). To avoid these negative outcomes, we must use our cognitive resources to plan, guide, and restrain

our behaviours. And we (like Gavin England) can also use our emotion regulation skills to help us do better.

Even in an age where the addictive and detrimental health effects of cigarette smoking are well understood,

more than 60% of children try smoking before they are 18 years old, and more than half who have smoked

have tried and failed to quit (Fryar, Merino, Hirsch, & Porter, 2009). Although smoking is depicted in movies

as sexy and alluring, it is highly addictive and probably the most dangerous thing we can do to our body.

Poor diet and physical inactivity combine to make up the second greatest threat to our health. But we can

improve our diet by eating more natural and less processed food, and by monitoring our food intake. And we

can start and maintain an exercise program. Exercise keeps us happier, improves fitness, and leads to better

health and lower mortality (Fogelholm, 2010; Galper, Trivedi, Barlow, Dunn, & Kampert, 2006; Hassm.n,

Koivula, & Uutela, 2000). And exercise also has a variety of positive influences on our cognitive processes,

including academic performance (Hillman, Erickson, & Kramer, 2008).

Alcohol abuse, and particularly binge drinking (i.e., having five or more drinks in one sitting), is often the

norm among high school and university students, but it has severe negative health consequences. Bingeing

leads to deaths from car crashes, drowning, falls, gunshots, and alcohol poisoning (Valencia-Mart.n, Gal.n,

& Rodr.guez-Artalejo, 2008). Binge-drinking students are also more likely to be involved in other risky

behaviours, such as smoking, drug use, dating violence, or attempted suicide (Miller, Naimi, Brewer, &

Jones, 2007). Binge drinking may also damage neural pathways in the brain (McQueeny et al., 2009) and

lead to lifelong alcohol abuse and dependency (Kim et al., 2008). Illicit drug use has also been increasing

and is linked to the spread of infectious diseases such as HIV, hepatitis B, and hepatitis C (Monteiro, 2001).

Some teens abstain from sex entirely, particularly those who are very religious, but most experiment with it.

In Canada, 30% of 15- to 17-year-olds and 68% of 18- to 19-year-olds reported that they had had intercourse

(Rotermann, 2012). In sum, while less than half of Canadian teens report having intercourse before age 18,

more than two-thirds do so before age 20. When these data are added to data from previous cycles of the

Canadian Community Health Survey we can see that the percentages of Canadian young people in the 15-

to 17- and 18- to 19-year-old age groups who reported ever having sexual intercourse remained remarkably

stable from 1996/1997 to 2009/2010. Although sex is fun, it can also kill us if we are not careful. Sexual

activity can lead to guilt about having engaged in the act itself, and may also lead to unwanted pregnancies

and sexually transmitted infections (STIs), including HIV infection. Alcohol consumption also leads to risky

sexual behaviour. Sex partners who have been drinking are less likely to practice safe sex, and they have

an increased risk of STIs, including HIV infection (Hutton, McCaul, Santora, & Erbelding 2008; Raj et al.,

2009).

It takes some work to improve and maintain our health and happiness, and our desire for the positive

emotional experiences that come from engaging in dangerous behaviours can get in the way of this work. But

being aware of the dangers, working to control our emotions, and using our resources to engage in healthy

behaviours and avoid unhealthy ones are the best things we can do for ourselves.

473 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Key Takeaways

• Biologically, hunger is controlled by the interactions among complex pathways in the nervous

system and a variety of hormonal and chemical systems in the brain and body.

• How we eat is also influenced by our environment, including social norms about appropriate body

size.

• Homeostasis varies among people and is determined by the basal metabolic rate. Low metabolic

rates, which are determined entirely by genetics, make weight management a very difficult

undertaking for many people.

• Eating disorders, including anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa, affect about 1% of people

(90% women).

• Obesity is a medical condition in which so much excess body fat has accumulated in the body that

it begins to have an adverse impact on health. Uncontrolled obesity leads to health problems

including cardiovascular disease, diabetes, sleep apnea, arthritis, and some types of cancer.

• The two approaches to controlling weight are to eat less and exercise more.

• Sex drive is regulated by the sex hormones estrogen in women and testosterone in both women

and men.

• Although their biological determinants and experiences of sex are similar, men and women differ

substantially in their overall interest in sex, the frequency of their sexual activities, and the mates

they are most interested in.

• Sexual behaviour varies widely, not only between men and women but also within each sex.

• There is also variety in sexual orientation: toward people of the opposite sex, people of the same

sex, or people of both sexes. The determinants of sexual orientation are primarily biological.

• We can outwit stress, obesity, and other health risks through appropriate healthy action.

Exercise and Critical Thinking

1. Consider your own eating and sex patterns. Are they healthy or unhealthy? What can you do to

improve them?

References

American Heart Association. (1998). Statement on exercise, benefits and recommendations for physical activity

programs for all Americans. American Heart Association, 94, 857–862. Retrieved from http://circ.ahajournals.org/

cgi/content/full/94/4/857?ijkey=6e9ad2e53ba5b25f9002a707e5e4b5b8ee015481&keytype2=tf_ipsecsha.

Aversa, A., Isidori, A., De Martino, M., Caprio, M., Fabbrini, E., Rocchietti-March, M.,…Fabri, A. (2000).

11.4 TWO FUNDAMENTAL HUMAN MOTIVATIONS: EATING AND MATING • 474

Androgens and penile erection: evidence for a direct relationship between free testosterone and cavernous

vasodilation in men with erectile dysfunction. Clinical Endocrinology, 53(4), 517–522.

Bailey, J., Pillard, R., Dawood, K., Miller, M., Farrer, L., Shruti Trivedi, L.,…Murphy, R. L. (1999). A family

history study of male sexual orientation using three independent samples. Behavior Genetics, 29(2), 79–86.

Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The female sex drive as socially flexible and

responsive. Psychological Bulletin, 126(3), 347–374.

Bogaert, A. (2004). Asexuality: Prevalence and associated factors in a national probability sample. Journal of Sex

Research, 41(3), 279–287. Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

Borer, K. T. (2008). How effective is exercise in producing fat loss? Kinesiology, 40(2), 126–137.

Brennan, A. M., & Mantzoros, C. S. (2006). Drug insight: The role of leptin in human physiology and

pathophysiology-emerging clinical applications. Nature Clinical Practice Endocrinology Metabolism, 2(6),

318–27.

Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37

cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12(1), 1–49.

Buss, D., & Kenrick, D. (1998). Evolutionary social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey

(Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 982–1026). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

Colley, R. C., Garriguet, D., Janssen, I., Craig, C. L., Clarke, J., & Tremblay, M. S. (2011). Physical activity of

Canadian adults: Accelerometer results from the 2007 to 2009 Canadian Health Measures Survey. Statistics

Canada.

Crandall, C. S. (1988). Social contagion of binge eating. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 55(4),

588–598.

Crandall, C. S., Merman, A., & Hebl, M. (2009). Anti-fat prejudice. In T. D. Nelson (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice,

stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 469–487). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Douketis, J. D., Macie C., Thabane, L., & Williamson, D. F. (2005). Systematic review of long-term weight loss

studies in obese adults: Clinical significance and applicability to clinical practice. International Journal of Obesity,

29, 1153–1167.

Feldhaus-Dahir, M. (2009). The causes and prevalence of hypoactive sexual desire disorder: Part I. Urologic

Nursing, 29(4), 259–263. Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

Fogelholm, M. (2010). Physical activity, fitness and fatness: Relations to mortality, morbidity and disease risk

factors. A systematic review. Obesity Reviews, 11(3), 202–221.

Fryar, C. D., Merino, M. C., Hirsch, R., & Porter, K. S. (2009). Smoking, alcohol use, and illicit drug use reported

by adolescents aged 12–17 years: United States, 1999–2004. National Health Statistics Reports, 15, 1–23.

Galper, D., Trivedi, M., Barlow, C., Dunn, A., & Kampert, J. (2006). Inverse association between physical inactivity

and mental health in men and women. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 38(1), 173–178.

475 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Gladue, B. A. (1994). The biopsychology of sexual orientation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(5),

150–154.

Guisinger, S. (2008). Competing paradigms for anorexia nervosa. American Psychologist, 63(3), 199–200.

Gustafson, D., Rothenberg, E., Blennow, K., Steen, B., & Skoog, I. (2003). An 18-year follow-up of overweight

and risk of Alzheimer disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 163(13), 1524.

Haslam, D. W., & James, W. P. (2005). Obesity. Lancet, 366(9492), 197–209.

Hassm.n, P., Koivula, N., & Uutela, A. (2000). Physical exercise and psychological well-being: A population study

in Finland. Preventive Medicine: An International Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory, 30(1), 17–25.

Hershberger, S., & Segal, N. (2004). The cognitive, behavioral, and personality profiles of a male monozygotic

triplet set discordant for sexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33(5), 497–514. Retrieved from Academic

Search Premier Database.

Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain

and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(1), 58–65.

Hoek, H. W., & van Hoeken, D. (2003). Review of the prevalence and incidence of eating disorders. International

Journal of Eating Disorders, 34(4), 383–396.

Hutton, H., McCaul, M., Santora, P., & Erbelding, E. (2008). The relationship between recent alcohol use and

sexual behaviors: Gender differences among sexually transmitted disease clinic patients. Alcoholism: Clinical &

Experimental Research, 32(11), 2008–2015.

James, W. P. (2008). The fundamental drivers of the obesity epidemic. Obesity Review, 9(Suppl. 1), 6–13.

Jockenh.vel, F., Minnemann, T., Schubert, M., Freude, S., Hübler, D., Schumann, C.,…Ernst, M. (2009). Timetable

of effects of testosterone administration to hypogonadal men on variables of sex and mood. Aging Male, 12(4),

113–118.

Keys, A., Brožek, J., Henschel, A., Mickelsen, O., & Taylor, H. L. (1950). The biology of human starvation (Vols.

1–2). Oxford, England: University of Minnesota Press.

Kim, J., Sing, L., Chow, J., Lau, J., Tsang, A., Choi, J.,…Griffiths, S. M. (2008). Prevalence and the factors

associated with binge drinking, alcohol abuse, and alcohol dependence: A population-based study of Chinese adults

in Hong Kong. Alcohol & Alcoholism, 43(3), 360–370.

Kingston, D. A., & Firestone, P. (2008). Problematic hypersexuality: A review of conceptualization and

diagnosis. Sexual Addictions and Compulsivity, 15, 284–310.

Kinsey, A. C. (1998). Sexual behavior in the human female. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. (Original work

published 1953)

Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1998). Sexual behavior in the human male. Bloomington: Indiana

University Press. (Original work published 1948).

Kirchhoff, A., Elliott, L., Schlichting, J., & Chin, M. (2008). Strategies for physical activity maintenance in

African American women. American Journal of Health Behavior, 32(5), 517–524. Retrieved from Academic Search

Premier Database.

11.4 TWO FUNDAMENTAL HUMAN MOTIVATIONS: EATING AND MATING • 476

Klump, K. L., Burt, S. A., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). Changes in genetic and environmental influences

on disordered eating across adolescence: A longitudinal twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(12),

1409–1415.

Kramer, A. F., Erickson, K. I., & Colcombe, S. J. (2006). Exercise, cognition, and the aging brain. Journal of

Applied Physiology, 101(4), 1237–1242.

Kulkin, H. S., Chauvin, E. A., & Percle, G. A. (2000). Suicide among gay and lesbian adolescents and young adults:

A review of the literature. Journal of Homosexuality, 40(1), 1–29.

Lasco, M., Jordan, T., Edgar, M., Petito, C., & Byrne, W. (2002). A lack of dimorphism of sex or sexual orientation

in the human anterior commissure. Brain Research, 936(1/2), 95.

Laumann, E. O., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social organization of sexuality in the

United States. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Leitenberg, H., & Henning, K. (1995). Sexual fantasy. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 469–496.

Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human sexual response. New York, NY: Bantam Books.

McQueeny, T., Schweinsburg, B., Schweinsburg, A., Jacobus, J., Bava, S., Frank, L.,…Tapert, S. F. (2009).

Altered white matter integrity in adolescent binge drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 33(7),

1278–1285.

Meston, C. M., & Frohlich, P. F. (2000). The neurobiology of sexual function. Archives of General Psychiatry,

57(11), 1012–1030.

Miller, J., Naimi, T., Brewer, R., & Jones, S. (2007). Binge drinking and associated health risk behaviors among

high school students. Pediatrics, 119(1), 76–85.

Miller, W. C. (1999). How effective are traditional dietary and exercise interventions for weight loss? Medicine &

Science in Sports & Exercise, 31(8), 1129–1134.

Mokdad, A. H., Marks, J. S., Stroup, D. F., & Gerberding, J. L. (2004). Actual causes of death in the United States,

2000. Journal of the American Medical Association, 291(10), 1238–1240.

Monteiro, M. (2001). A World Health Organization perspective on alcohol and illicit drug use and health. European

Addiction Research, 7(3), 98–103.

Mustanski, B. S., Chivers, M. L., & Bailey, J. M. (2002). A critical review of recent biological research on human

sexual orientation. Annual Review of Sex Research, 13, 89–140.

Nakazato, M., Murakami, N., Date, Y., Kojima, M., Matsuo, H., Kangawa, K., & Matsukura S. (2001). A role for

ghrelin in the central regulation of feeding. Nature, 409(6817), 194–198.

Nestle, M., & Jacobson, M. F. (2000). Halting the obesity epidemic: A public health policy approach. Public Health

Reports, 115(1), 12–24.

Norton, K. I., Olds, T. S., Olive, S., & Dank, S. (1996). Ken and Barbie at life size. Sex Roles, 34(3–4), 287–294.

Palmer B. (2008) Epidemiology, diagnosing and assessing eating disorders. Psychiatry, 7(4):143-146.

477 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

Patrick, L. (2002). Eating disorders: A review of the literature with emphasis on medical complications and clinical

nutrition. Alternative Medicine Review, 7(3), 184–202.

Petersen, J. L., & Hyde, J. S. (2010). A meta-analytic review of research on gender differences in sexuality,

1993–2007. Psychological Bulletin, 136(1), 21–38.

Pillard, R., & Bailey, J. (1998). Human sexual orientation has a heritable component. Human Biology, 70(2), 347.

Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

Pillsworth, E., Haselton, M., & Buss, D. (2004). Ovulatory shifts in female sexual desire. Journal of Sex Research,

41(1), 55–65. Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

Rahman, Q., & Wilson, G. D. (2003). Born gay? The psychobiology of human sexual orientation. Personality and

Individual Differences, 34(8), 1337–1382.

Raj, A., Reed, E., Santana, M., Walley, A., Welles, S., Horsburgh, C.,…Silverman, J. G. (2009). The associations

of binge alcohol use with HIV/STI risk and diagnosis among heterosexual African American men. Drug & Alcohol

Dependence, 101(1/2), 101–106.

Rotermann, M. (2012). Sexual behaviour and condom use of 15- to 24-year olds in 2003 and 2009/2010 [PDF].

Statistics Canada, Catalogue no. 82-003-XPE. Health Reports, 23(1). Retrieved from http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/

82-003-x/2012001/article/11632-eng.pdf

Rozin, P., Dow, S., Moscovitch, M., & Rajaram, S. (1998). What causes humans to begin and end a meal? A role for

memory for what has been eaten, as evidenced by a study of multiple meal eating in amnesic patients. Psychological

Science, 9(5), 392–396.in 2003 and

Saad, G., Eba, A., & Sejean, R. (2009). Sex differences when searching for a mate: A process-tracing

approach. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 22(2), 171–190.

Schachter, S. (1968). Obesity and eating. Science, 161(3843), 751–756.

Schomer, H., & Drake, B. (2001). Physical activity and mental health. International SportMed Journal, 2(3), 1.

Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

Statistics Canada. (2010). Popularity of Physical Recreation Activities of Adults, Age 20+

(Canadian Community Health Survey). Available at: http://www.cflri.ca/eng/levels/popular_pa_adults.php.

Accessed May 22, 2014.

Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. (1995). Human female orgasm and mate fluctuating asymmetry. Animal Behaviour,

50(6), 1601. Retrieved from Academic Search Premier Database.

Valencia-Mart.n, J., Gal.n, I., & Rodr.guez-Artalejo, F. (2008). The joint association of average volume of alcohol

and binge drinking with hazardous driving behaviour and traffic crashes. Addiction, 103(5), 749–757.

Vohs, K. D., Voelz, Z. R., Pettit, J. W., Bardone, A. M., Katz, J., Abramson, L. Y.,…Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2001).

Perfectionism, body dissatisfaction, and self-esteem: An interactive model of bulimic symptom

development. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 20(4), 476–497.

West, S. L., D’Aloisio, A. A., Agans, R. P., Kalsbeek, W. D., Borisov, N. N., & Thorp, J. M. (2008). Prevalence

11.4 TWO FUNDAMENTAL HUMAN MOTIVATIONS: EATING AND MATING • 478

of low sexual desire and hypoactive sexual desire disorder in a nationally representative sample of US

women. Archives of Internal Medicine, 168(13), 1441–1449.

Williams, T., & Pepitone, M. (2000) Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation. Nature, 404, 455.

Wolf, G., & Miller, N. E. (1964). Lateral hypothalamic lesions: Effects on drinking elicited by carbachol in preoptic

area and posterior hypothalamus. Science, 143(Whole No. 3606), 585–587.

World Health Organization. (2010). World Health Organization. Global Recommendations on Physical Activity

for Health. Geneva, World Health Organization.

Yarema, M. (2013). Canada must respect the right to food. Huffington Post, Canada. Retrieved 2014

from http://www.huffingtonpost.ca/megan-yarema/olivier-de-schutter_b_2814600.html

Long Descriptions

Figure 11.11 long description: Biological, Psychological, and Social-Cultural Contributors to Eating Behaviour

Biological influences: Psychological influences: Social-cultural influences:

• hypothalamus sends cues to start or stop

eating

• the appetite hormones, insulim, leptin,

orexin, and ghrelin, control hunger and

eating

• individual differences in basal metabolic

rate

• sight and smell of food

• time elapsed since last meal

• individual differences in

self-esteem, mood,

perfectionism

• norms about

appropriate body

weight

• culturally

preferred and

available foods

[Return to Figure 11.11]

479 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION

11.5 Chapter Summary

Affect guides behaviour, helps us make decisions, and has a major impact on our mental and physical health. Affect

is guided by arousal—our experiences of the bodily responses created by the sympathetic division of the autonomic

nervous system.

Emotions are the mental and physiological feeling states that direct our attention and guide our behaviour. The

most fundamental emotions, known as the basic emotions, are those of anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness,

and surprise. A variety of secondary emotions are determined by the process of cognitive appraisal. The distinction

between the primary and the secondary emotions is paralleled by two brain pathways: a fast pathway and a slow

pathway.

There are three primary theories of emotion, each supported by research evidence. The Cannon-Bard theory of

emotion proposes that the experience of an emotion is accompanied by physiological arousal. The James-Lange

theory of emotion proposes that our experience of an emotion is the result of the arousal that we experience. The

two-factor theory of emotion asserts that the experience of emotion is determined by the intensity of the arousal

we are experiencing, but that the cognitive appraisal of the situation determines what the emotion will be. When

people incorrectly label the source of the arousal that they are experiencing, we say that they have misattributed

their arousal.

We communicate and perceive emotion in part through nonverbal communication and through facial expressions.

The facial feedback hypothesis proposes that we also experience emotion in part through our own facial expressions.

Stress refers to the physiological responses that occur when an organism fails to respond appropriately to emotional

or physical threats. When it is extreme or prolonged, stress can create substantial health problems.

The general adaptation syndrome describes the three phases of physiological change that occur in response to

long-term stress: alarm, resistance, and exhaustion. Stress creates a long-term negative effect on the body by

activating the HPA axis, which produces the stress hormone cortisol. The HPA reactions to persistent stress lead to

a weakening of the immune system. Chronic stress is also a major contributor to heart disease.

The stress that we experience in our everyday lives, including daily hassles, can be taxing. People who experience

strong negative emotions as a result of these hassles exhibit more negative stress responses those who react in a less

negative way.

On average, men are more likely than women are to respond to stress by activating the fight-or-flight response,

whereas women are more likely to respond using the tend-and-befriend response.

Attempting to ignore or suppress our stressors is not effective, in part because it is difficult to do. It is healthier to

let out the negative thoughts and feelings by expressing them, either to ourselves or to others. It is easier to respond

to stress if we can interpret it in more positive ways — for instance, as a challenge rather than a threat.

The ability to successfully control our emotions is known as emotion regulation. Regulating emotions takes effort,

but the ability to do so can have important positive health outcomes.

The best antidote for stress is to think positively, have fun, and enjoy the company of others. People who express

480

optimism, self-efficacy, and hardiness cope better with stress and experience better health overall. Happiness is

determined in part by genetic factors such that some people are naturally happier than others, but it is also facilitated

by social support — our positive social relationships with others.

People often do not know what will make them happy. After a minimum level of wealth is reached, more money

does not generally buy more happiness. Although people think that positive and negative events will make a huge

difference in their lives, and although these changes do make at least some difference in life satisfaction, they tend

to be less influential than we think they are going to be.

A motivation is a driving force that initiates and directs behaviour. Motivations are often considered in psychology

in terms of drives and goals, with the goal of maintaining homeostasis.

Eating is a primary motivation determined by hormonal and social factors. Cultural norms about appropriate weights

influence eating behaviours. The desire to be thin can lead to eating disorders including anorexia nervosa and

bulimia nervosa.

Uncontrolled obesity leads to health problems including cardiovascular disease, diabetes, sleep apnea, arthritis,

Alzheimer’s disease, and some types of cancer. It is a leading preventable cause of death worldwide. The two

approaches to controlling weight are eating less and exercising more.

Sex is a fundamental motivation that involves the coordination of a wide variety of behaviours, including courtship,

sex, household arrangements, parenting, and child care. The sexual response cycle is similar in men and women.

The sex hormone testosterone is particularly important for sex drive, in both men and women.

Sexual behaviour varies widely, not only between men and women but within each sex.

The vast majority of human beings have a heterosexual orientation, but a smaller minority are primarily homosexual

or bisexual. The love and sexual lives of homosexuals and bisexuals are little different from those of heterosexuals,

except where their behaviours are constrained by cultural norms and local laws.

481 • INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY - 1ST CANADIAN EDITION


Last modified: Thursday, May 26, 2022, 9:48 AM